While a significant amount of research has focussed on whether bilingualism bestows advantages in cognitive skills, perspective-taking and Theory of Mind, less is known about the effect of bilingualism in communicative tasks where these and related skills may be called for. This study examines bilingual and monolingual adults’ communicative skills through their production of two types of listener-adapted speech (LAS): child-directed speech and foreigner-directed speech. 20 monolinguals and 20 bilingual adults were asked to explain a cooking recipe to a child, a non-native adult and a control native adult. Participants adapted their speech for the child and the foreigner compared to the native adult. Furthermore, bilinguals adapted some features of their speech to a greater extent and in a fine-tuned way (wider pitch range addressing the child and vowel hyperarticulation addressing the foreigner). The prevalence of these features in bilingual speech was not correlated with personality or cognitive measures. We discuss possible sources of this difference in speech adaptation and implications for theories of bilingual cognition.
Allison, C., Auyeung, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Toward Brief “Red Flags” for Autism Screening: The Short Autism Spectrum Quotient and the Short Quantitative Checklist in 1,000 Cases and 3,000 Controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(2), 202–212.e7.
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., … Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398.
Antoniou, K., Kambanaros, M., Grohmann, K. K., & Katsos, N. (2014). Is bilectalism similar to bilingualism? An investigation into children’s vocabulary and executive control skills. In BUCLD 38: Proceedings of the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 11, pp. 12–24). Cascadilla Press Somerville, MA.
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual Effects on Cognitive and Linguistic Development: Role of Language, Cultural Background, and Education. Child Development, 83(2),
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48(3), 1009–1018.
Biersack, S., Biersack, S., Kempe, V., & Knapton, L. (2005). FineTuning Speech Registers: A Comparison of the Prosodic Features of Child-Directed and Foreigner-Directed Speech. In: Proceedings of the 9th European conference on speech communication and technology.
Bradlow, A. R. (2002). Confluent talker- and listener-related forces in clear speech production. Mouton de Gruyter. Retrieved from [URL]
Bradlow, A. R., & Bent, T. (2002). The clear speech effect for non-native listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(1), 272–284.
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., McLean, J. F., & Brown, A. (2011). The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers. Cognition, 121(1), 41–57.
Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2002). What’s new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science (New York, N.Y.), 296(5572), 1435.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298.
Culbertson, G. H., & Caporael, L. R. (1983). Baby Talk Speech to the Elderly. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(2), 305–312.
DePaulo, B. M., & Coleman, L. M. (1986). Talking to children, foreigners, and retarded adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 945–59.
Dewaele, J. -M., & van Oudenhoven, J. P. (2009). The effect of multilingualism/multiculturalism on personality: no gain without pain for Third Culture Kids?International Journal of Multilingualism, 6(4), 443–459.
Dewaele, J. -M., & Wei, L. (2012). Multilingualism, empathy and multicompetence. International Journal of Multilingualism, 9(4), 352–366.
Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. -J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The Inhibitory Advantage in Bilingual Children Revisited. Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234–251.
Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The Exposure Advantage. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1090–1097.
Farwell, C. B., Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1979). Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Language, 55(2), 449.
Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register Snow, CE FergusonC. A. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to Children. Cambridge University Press.
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby Talk in Six Languages. American Anthropologist, 661 (6_PART2), 103–114.
Ferguson, S. H., & Kewley-Port, D. (2002). Vowel intelligibility in clear and conversational speech for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(1), 259–71. Retrieved from [URL]
Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and Disapproval: Infant Responsiveness to Vocal Affect in Familiar and Unfamiliar Languages. Child Development, 64(3), 657–674.
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16(3), 477.
Foltz, A., Gaspers, J., Thiele, K., Stenneken, P., & Cimiano, P. (2015). Lexical alignment in triadic communication. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 127.
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J. (1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge University Press.
Genesee, F., Tucker, G. R., & Lambert, W. E. (1975). Communication Skills of Bilingual Children. Child Development, 46(4), 1010–1014.
Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 1–15.
Goldman, M. C., Negen, J., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2014). Are bilingual children better at ignoring perceptually misleading information? A novel test. Developmental Science, 17(6), 956–964.
Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, 28(1), 41–50.
Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingual: Life and Reality. Sociolinguistic Studies, 61, 595–602.
Gullberg, M. (2012). Bilingualism and Gesture. In The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism (pp. 417–437). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Han, S., & Lee, K. (2013). Cognitive and Affective Perspective-Taking Ability of Young Bilinguals in South Korea. Child Studies in Diverse Contexts, 3(1), 69–80.
Hazan, V., & Markham, D. (2004). Acoustic-phonetic correlates of talker intelligibility for adults and children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(5), 3108–3118.
Hazan, V., & Baker, R. (2011). Acoustic-phonetic characteristics of speech produced with communicative intent to counter adverse listening conditions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(4), 2139–2152.
Katsos, N., Grohmann, K. K., Kambanaros, M., & Antoniou, K. (n.d.). Is bilectalism similar to bilingualism? An investigation into children’s vocabulary and executive control skills. Retrieved from [URL]
Kemper, S., Finter‐Urczyk, A., Ferrell, P., Harden, T., & Billington, C. (1998). Using elderspeak with older adults. Discourse Processes, 25(1), 55–73.
Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension. Psychological Science, 11(1), 32–38.
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2010). Kharkhurin: Bilingual verbal and nonverbal creativity Bilingual verbal and nonverbal creative behavior. International Journal of Bilingualism The International Journal of Bilingualisms, 14(142), 211–226.
Knoll, M., Uther, M., MacLeod, N., O’Neill, M., Walsh, S. (2006). Emotional, linguistic or just cute? The function of pitch contours in infant- and foreigner-directed speech. Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Speech Prosody, Dresden, Germany.
Kovács, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(16), 6556–60.
Kuhl, P. K. (1997). Cross-Language Analysis of Phonetic Units in Language Addressed to Infants. Science, 277(5326), 684–686.
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental Science, 20(1), 1–11.
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory. In Speech Production and Speech Modelling (pp. 403–439). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.
Mangold, R., & Pobel, R. (1988). Informativeness and Instrumentality in Referential Communication. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 7(3–4), 181–191.
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93.
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726.
Paap, K. R., Darrow, J., Dalibar, C., & Johnson, H. A. (2014). Effects of script similarity on bilingual advantages in executive control are likely to be negligible or null. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1539.
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258.
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015a). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 691, 265–278.
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015b). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 691, 265–278.
Pisoni, D. B., & Remez, R. E. (2008). The Handbook of Speech Perception. In The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 1–708). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Prato-Previde, E., Fallani, G., & Valsecchi, P. (2006). Gender Differences in Owners Interacting with Pet Dogs: An Observational Study. Ethology, 112(1), 64–73.
Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 203–212.
Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7(2), 353–69.
Rubio-Fernández, P. (2016). The director task: A test of Theory-of-Mind use or selective attention?Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
Rubio-Fernández, P., & Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people’s beliefs: Bilinguals have an advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 211–217.
Ryskin, R. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Yiu, L. K., & Nguyen, E. T. (2014). Visuospatial perspective-taking in conversation and the role of bilingual experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 741, 46–76.
Scarborough, R., Brenier, J. M., Zhao, Y., Hall-Lew, L., & Dmitrieva, O. (2007). An acoustic study of real and imagined foreigner-directed speech. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, (August), 2165–2168.
Siegal, M., Iozzi, L., & Surian, L. (2009). Bilingualism and conversational understanding in young children. Cognition, 110(1), 115–122.
Sullivan, M. D., Janus, M., Moreno, S., Astheimer, L., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Early stage second-language learning improves executive control: Evidence from ERP. Brain and Language, 1391, 84–98.
Uther, M., Knoll, M. A., & Burnham, D. (2007). Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication, 49(1), 2–7.
Walsh, S. (n.d.). Emotional, linguistic or just cute? The function of pitch contours in infant- and foreigner-directed speech. Retrieved from [URL]
Wassink, A. B., Wright, R. A., & Franklin, A. D. (2007). Intraspeaker variability in vowel production: An investigation of motherese, hyperspeech, and Lombard speech in Jamaican speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 351, 363–379.
Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2016). Children Increase Their Sensitivity to a Speaker’s Nonlinguistic Cues Following a Communicative Breakdown. Child Development, 87(2), 385–394.
Cited by (8)
Cited by eight other publications
Feng, Justin, Sohyun Cho & Gigi Luk
2023. Assessing Theory of Mind in bilinguals: A scoping review on tasks and study designs. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition► pp. 1 ff.
2023. Bilingual children display comparative strength using prosodic cues for pragmatic word learning. International Journal of Bilingualism
Rothermich, Kathrin, Rose Baker, Sharon M. Falkins, Jaeyoung Kum, Madison R. Capps & Susan C. Bobb
2023. Reported Use of Second-Language Speech Accommodation in Everyday Interactions: The Role of Individual Differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 66:9 ► pp. 3307 ff.
2022. Infants’ Lexical Processing: Independent Contributions of Attentional and Clarity Cues. Language Learning and Development► pp. 1 ff.
Piazza, Giorgio, Clara D. Martin & Marina Kalashnikova
2022. The Acoustic Features and Didactic Function of Foreigner-Directed Speech: A Scoping Review. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 65:8 ► pp. 2896 ff.
2022. Vowel acoustics of Nungon child-directed speech, adult dyadic conversation, and foreigner-directed monologues. Frontiers in Psychology 13
Kašćelan, Draško, Napoleon Katsos & Jenny L. Gibson
2019. Relations Between Bilingualism and Autistic-Like Traits in a General Population Sample of Primary School Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 49:6 ► pp. 2509 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.