Early Spanish-English bilinguals and English controls were tested on the production and perception of negative, short-lag, and long-lag Voice Onset Time (VOT), VOT types spanning the Spanish and English phonetic categories: phonologically, negative and short-lag VOT stops are distinct phonemes in Spanish, but realizations of voiced stops in English. Dominance was critical: more English-dominant bilinguals produced more short-lag VOT stops in response to negative VOT stimuli, and were also less accurate than more balanced bilinguals at discriminating negative from short-lag VOT. Bilinguals performed similarly to monolinguals overall, but they produced more negative VOT tokens and shorter short-lag VOT in response to negative VOT. Their productions were also less well correlated with perception and showed more variation between individuals. These results highlight the variable nature of bilingual production and perception, and demonstrate the need to consider language dominance, individual variation, as well as modalities and tasks when studying bilinguals.
Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice Onset Time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 631, 75–86.
Balukas, C., & Koops, C. (2014). Spanish-English bilingual voice onset time in spontaneous code-switching. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(4), 423–443.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.
Beach, E. F., Burnham, D., & Kitamura, C. (2001). Bilingualism and the relationship between perception and production: Greek/English bilinguals and Thai bilabial stops. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 5(2), 221–235.
Beddor, P. (2017). Speech perception in phonetics. Oxford research encyclopedia of linguistics. Retrieved 2 Nov. 2017, from [URL].
Best, C. T., & Strange, W. (1992). Effects of phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics, 20(3), 305–330.
Bills, G. D., Chávez, E. H., & Hudson, A. (1995). The geography of language shift: Distance from the Mexican border and Spanish language claiming in the Southwestern US. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 114(1), 9–28.
Cedrus Corporation. (2011). SuperLab 4.5. San Pedro, CA.
Cho, T., & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: Evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27(2), 207–229.
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1992). The monolingual nature of speech by bilinguals. Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 381–410.
Davidson, L. (2016). Variability in the implementation of voicing in American English obstruents. Journal of Phonetics, 541, 35–50.
Dmitrieva, O., Llanos, F., Shultz, A. A., & Francis, A. L. (2015). Phonological status, not voice onset time, determines the acoustic realization of onset f0 as a secondary voicing cue in Spanish and English. Journal of Phonetics, 491, 77–95.
Docherty, G. J. (1992). The timing of voicing in British English obstruents. Berlin/New York: Foris.
Dunn, A. L., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2009). A quick, gradient Bilingual Dominance Scale. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(3), 273–289.
Elman, J. L., Diehl, R. L., & Buchwald, S. E. (1977). Perceptual switching in bilinguals. The Journal of the acoustical Society of America, 62(4), 971–974.
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–273). Timonium, MD: York Press.
Flege, J. E., & Eefting, W. (1987). Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 151, 67–83.
Flege, J. E., & Eefting, W. (1988). Imitation of a VOT continuum by native speakers of English and Spanish: Evidence for phonetic category formation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83(2), 729–40.
Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. A., & Piske, T. (2002). Assessing bilingual dominance. Applied Psycholinguistics, 231, 567–598.
Fowler, C. A., Sramko, V., Ostry, D. J., Rowland, S. A., & Hallé, P. (2008). Cross language phonetic influences on the speech of French-English bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics, 36(4), 649–663.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81.
Grijalva, C., Piccinini, P., & Arvaniti, A. (2013). The vowel spaces of Southern California English and Mexican Spanish as produced by monolinguals and bilinguals. POMA (Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics), 19(1), 060088.
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 361, 3–15.
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 131–149.
Grosjean, F. (2001). The bilingual’s language modes. In Nicol, J. (Ed.). One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 1–22). Oxford: Blackwell. Also in Li Wei (Ed.). The bilingual reader (2nd edition). London: Routledge, 2007.
Keating, P. A. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60(2), 286–319.
Kessinger, R. H., & Blumstein, S. E. (1997). Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time in Thai, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25(2), 143–168.
Lengeris, A., & Hazan, V. (2011). The effect of native vowel processing ability and frequency discrimination acuity on the phonetic training of English vowels for native speakers of Greek. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1281, 3757–3768.
Lipski, J. M. (2008). Varieties of Spanish in the United States. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422.
Mack, M. (1989). Consonant and vowel perception and production: Early English-French bilinguals and English monolinguals. Perception & Psychophysics, 46(2), 187–200.
MacKay, I. R., Flege, J. E., Piske, T., & Schirru, C. (2001). Category restructuring during second-language speech acquisition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110(1), 516–528.
Magloire, J., & Green, K. P. (1999). A cross-language comparison of speaking rate effects on the production of Voice Onset Time in English and Spanish. Phonetica, 56(3–4), 158–185.
McGuire, G. (2010). A brief primer on experimental designs for speech perception research. Available at [URL]
Nakai, S., & Scobbie, J. M. (2016). The VOT category boundary in word-initial stops: Counter-Evidence against rate normalization in English spontaneous speech. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology, 7(1), 13.
Olson, D. J. (2013). Bilingual language switching and selection at the phonetic level: Asymmetrical transfer in VOT production. Journal of Phonetics, 41(6), 407–420.
Piccinini, P., & Arvaniti, A. (2015). Voice onset time in Spanish – English spontaneous code-switching. Journal of Phonetics, 521, 121–137.
Pisoni, D. (1973). Auditory and phonetic memory codes in the discrimination of consonants and vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 13(2), 253–260.
Raphael, L. J., Borden, G. J., & Harris, K. S. (2007). Speech science primer: Physiology, acoustics, and perception of speech. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Stuart-Smith, J., Sonderegger, M., Rathcke, T., & Macdonald, R. (2015). The private life of stops: VOT in a real-time corpus of spontaneous Glaswegian. Laboratory Phonology, 6(3–4), 505–549.
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Baum, S. (2006). Production of coronal stops by simultaneous bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(1), 97–114.
Theodore, R. M., Miller, J. L., & DeSteno, D. (2009). Individual talker differences in voice-onset-time: Contextual influences. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), 3974–3982.
Williams, L. (1977). The perception of stop consonant voicing by Spanish-English bilinguals. Perception & Psychophysics, 21(4), 289–297.
Yu, A., Abrego-Collier, C., & Sonderegger, M. (2013). Phonetic imitation from an individual-difference perspective: Subjective attitude, personality and “autistic” traits. PLOS ONE 8(9): e74746. Published: September 30, 2013.
Cited by (9)
Cited by nine other publications
Duarte-Borquez, Claudia, Maxine Van Doren & Marc Garellek
2024. Utterance-Final Voice Quality in American English and Mexican Spanish Bilinguals. Languages 9:3 ► pp. 70 ff.
Wrembel, Magdalena, Ulrike Gut, Romana Kopečková & Anna Balas
2024. The relationship between the perception and production of L2 and L3 rhotics in young multilinguals; an exploratory cross-linguistic study. International Journal of Multilingualism 21:1 ► pp. 92 ff.
2023. Perception of ATR contrasts by Akan speakers: a case of perceptual near-merger. Laboratory Phonology
Genitrini, Matteo, Julian Fritz, Georg Zimmermann & Hermann Schwameder
2022. Downhill Sections Are Crucial for Performance in Trail Running Ultramarathons—A Pacing Strategy Analysis. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology 7:4 ► pp. 103 ff.
Kim, Ji Young & Gemma Repiso-Puigdelliura
2020. Deconstructing Heritage Language Dominance: Effects of Proficiency, Use, and Input on Heritage Speakers’ Production of the Spanish Alveolar Tap. Phonetica 77:1 ► pp. 55 ff.
Azpiroz, Maialen Iraola, Shanley E. M. Allen, Kalliopi Katsika & Leigh Fernandez
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.