References (11)
References
Adelman, J. S., & G. D. A. Brown. (2008). Modeling lexical decision: The form of frequency and diversity effects. Psychological Review, 115(1), 214–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., & J. L. McClelland. (2005). Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. The Linguistic Review, 22(2–4), 381–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Einstein, A. (1919). Induction and deduction in physics. Berliner Tageblatt, (December 25).Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In R. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 257–287). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Kapatsinski, V. (2018). Words versus rules (Storage versus on-line production/processing) in morphology. In M. Aronoff (Ed.), Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Velar palatalization in Russian and artificial grammar: Constraints on models of morphophonology. Laboratory Phonology, 1(2), 361–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76(2), 165–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., P. Hendrix, C. Shaoul, P. Milin, & H. Baayen. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(1), 5–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rouder, J. N., F. Tuerlinckx, P. Speckman, J. Lu, & P. Gomez. (2008). A hierarchical approach for fitting curves to response time measurements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1201–1208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. (2016). The price of linguistic productivity: How children learn to break the rules of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). A formalist perspective on language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 665–706.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

HENKE, Ryan E.
2023. Rules and exceptions: A Tolerance Principle account of the possessive suffix in Northern East Cree. Journal of Child Language 50:5  pp. 1119 ff. DOI logo
Enger, Hans-Olav
2022. Type frequency is not the only factor that determines productivity, so the Tolerance Principle is not enough. Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 144:2  pp. 161 ff. DOI logo
Kapatsinski, Vsevolod
2022. Morphology in a Parallel, Distributed, Interactive Architecture of Language Production. Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence 5 DOI logo
Goldberg, Adele E.
2018. The sufficiency principle hyperinflates the price of productivity. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8:6  pp. 727 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.