References
Alhemaid, A.
(2018) The perception and production of English initial sC(C) clusters by Saudi ESL learners. 10th Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching (PSLLT) Conference , Ames, Iowa.
Archibald, J. & N. Croteau
under revision). Phonological but not syntactic contiguity in L2 Japanese Wh questions. Second Language Research.
Archibald, J. & Libben, G.
(2019) Morphological theory and second language acquisition. In F. Masini & Audring, J. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of morphological theory (pp. 522–540). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Archibald, J.
(2019) Multiple exponence in L2 German plural allomorphy: feature dependency and the poverty of the stimulus. March. Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition. Reno, Nevada.Google Scholar
Archibald, J. & Yousefi, M.
(2018) The redeployment of marked L1 Persian codas in the acquisition of marked L2 English onsets: Redeployment as a transition theory. Conference on Central Asian Language and Linguistics. Bloomington, Indiana.
Archibald, J.
(2019) A unified model of mono- and bilingual intelligibility: Psycholinguistics meets pedagogy. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech 1(1), 8–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) The acquisition of L2 phrasal stress. In M. Young-Scholten and S. J. Hannah (Eds.), Focus on phonological acquisition (pp. 263–290). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar
Dresher, E.
(1999) Charting the learning path: Cues to parameter setting. Linguistic Inquiry, 301, 27–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goad, H.
(2016) Phonotactic evidence from typology and acquisition for a coda+onset analysis of initial sC clusters. In Kim et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of 33rd WCCFL. Cascadilla Press. pp. 17–28.Google Scholar
Goad, H. & White, L.
(2019) Prosodic Effects on L2 Grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9(6). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gregg, K.
(1993) Taking explanation seriously; or, Let a couple of flowers bloom. Applied Linguistics, 14(3), 276–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleo, C. & Vogel, I.
(2004) Learning new segments and reducing domains in German L2 phonology: the role of the Prosodic Hierarchy. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8(1), 79–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özcelik, Ö.
(2016) The prosodic acquisition path hypothesis: Towards explaining variability in L2 acquisition of phonology. Glossa, 1(1), 28–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P.
(1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4), 707–784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stefanich, S., Cabrelli Amaro, J., Hilderman, D. & Archibald, J.
under revision). The morphophonology of intraword codeswitching. Frontiers in Communication: Language Sciences.
Vaux, B., & Wolfe, A.
(2009) The appendix. In E. Raimy & C. Cairns (Eds.), Contemporary views on architecture and representations in phonology (pp. 101–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2018) A formalist perspective on language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 665–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young Scholten, M.
(1993) The acquisition of prosodic structure in a second language. Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar