References

References

Chomsky, N.
(1986) Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Conroy, A., Takahashi, E., Lidz, J., & Phillips, C.
2009Equal treatment for all antecedents: How children succeed with Principle B. Linguistic Inquiry, 40, 446–486. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., & McKee, C.
(1986) The acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. In S. Berman, J.-W. Choe, & J. McDonough (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th North Eastern Linguistics Society (pp. 94–110). Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Mazuka, R., Jincho, N., & Oishi, H.
(2009) Development of executive control and language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 59–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E.L., & Aslin, R.N.
(2004) Learning at a distance: I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 127–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Omaki, A.
(2010) Commitment and flexibility in the developing parser. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland.
Saffran, J.R.
(2003) Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2004) The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 238–299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W.
(2007) Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stager, C.L., & Werker, J.F.
(1997) Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381–382. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tavakolian, S.
(1978) Children’s comprehension of pronominal subjects and missing subjects in complicated sentences. In H. Goodluck & L. Solan (Eds.), Papers in the structure and development of child language: University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics #4. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.[ p. 286 ]Google Scholar
Wijnen, F.
in press). Acquisition of linguistic categories: Cross-domain convergences. In J. Boluis & M. Everaert Eds. Birdsong, speech and language: Converging mechanisms Cambridge, MA MIT Press
Yoshida, K., Fennell, C., Swingley, D., & Werker, J.F.
(2009) 14-month-olds learn phonetically similar words. Developmental Science, 12, 412–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Dyson, Bronwen Patricia
2016. Variation, individual differences and second language processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:4  pp. 341 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.