This study investigated the effects of cross-linguistic influence in Japanese speakers’ integration of
morphological and syntactic information during the processing of Korean transitive causative constructions. We examined whether
Japanese speakers would process two types of Korean causative constructions as efficiently as native speakers: (a) when one target
structure was instantiated differently from learners’ L1 correspondents and (b) when the other type was unique to the L2. Although
the learners showed native-like performance during an acceptability judgment task, they had difficulties with the integration of
morphological and syntactic information during a self-paced reading task when the target construction gave rise to
cross-linguistic competitions with the L1 correspondent, but not when the target construction was unique to the L1. Our findings
support the claim that cross-linguistic cue competitions are a major source of difficulties in L2 sentence processing.
Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2009). An overview of semantic processing in bilinguals: Methods and findings. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 79–98). Multilingual Matters.
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data. A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge University Press.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278.
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J. & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 161–174.
Choe, J. (2014). Acquisition of the mapping between the causation type and the causative form in Korean. Korean Journal of Linguistics, 391, 641–653.
Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). A Tutorial on Fitting Cumulative Link Models with the Ordinal Package. Retrieved from [URL]
Christensen, R. H. B., & Brockhof, P. B. (2013). Analysis of sensory ratings data with cumulative link models. Journal de la Société Française de Statistique, 1541, 58–79.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 107–126.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2018). Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 401, 693–706.
Comrie, B. (1989). Language universals and linguistic typology. University of Chicago Press.
Cunnings, I. (2017). Parsing and working memory in bilingual sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 201, 659–678.
Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 179–201). Oxford University Press.
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 51, 175–197.
Drummond, A. (2013). Ibex Farm. Retrieved from [URL]
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 351, 353–387.
Dussias, P. E., & Piñar, P. (2010). Effects of reading span and plausibility in the reanalysis of wh-gaps by Chinese-English second language
speakers. Second Language Research, 261, 443–472.
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 101–116.
Ellis, R. (2002). Does form-focused instruction affect the acquisition of implicit knowledge?Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 241, 223–236.
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference,
overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 271, 164–194.
Ellis, N., & Sagarra, N. (2010). The bounds of adult language acquisition: Blocking and learned attention. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 321, 553–580.
Ferreira, F. & Clifton, Jr, C. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 251, 348–68.
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2002). An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language. In J. Altarriba & R. Herridia (Eds.), Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 218–236). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 681, 1–76.
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem?Second Language Research, 281, 191–215.
Harada, S. I. (1973). Counter Equi NP deletion. Annual Bulletin, 71, 113–147.
Harada, S. I. (1975). The functional uniqueness principle. Attempts in Linguistics and Literature, 21, 17–24.
Jeong, J. (2017). A Study on the L2 Korean Acquisition of Tense, Aspect, and Modality Pre-final Endings by L1 Chinese
Learners. Ph.D. dissertation, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, South Korea.
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 603–634.
Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 571, 1–33.
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes and in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 31, 228–238.
Kaan, E., Ballantyne, J. C., & Wijnen, F. (2015). Effects of reading speed on second-language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 361, 799–830.
Kim, H., & Grüter, T. (2019). Cross-linguistic activation of implicit causality biases in Korean learners of English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 221, 441–455.
Kruschke, J. K., & Blair, N. J. (2000). Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 71, 636–645.
Lago, S., Mosca, M., & Stutter Garcia, A. (2020). The Role of Crosslinguistic Influence in Multilingual Processing: Lexicon Versus Syntax. Language Learning. Published online by Wiley Periodicals, May22, 2020.
Le Pelley, M. E., Haselgrove, M., & Esber, G. R. (2012). Modeling attention in associative learning: Two processes or one?Learning & Behavior, 401, 292–304.
Luk, Z. P., & Shirai, Y. (2009). Is the acquisition order of grammatical morphemes impervious to L1 knowledge? Evidence from the acquisition of
plural –s, articles and possessive ‘s. Language Learning, 591, 721–754.
Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2005). Gaps in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 271, 53–78.
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The Competition Model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249–308). Lawrence Erlbaum.
MacWhinney, B. (2008). A unified model. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 341–371). Routledge.
MacWhinney, B. (2013). The logic of the unified model. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 211–227). New York: Routledge.
Paul, J. Z., & Grüter, T. (2016). Blocking effects in the learning of Chinese classifiers. Language Learning, 661, 972–999.
Pritchett, B. L. (1991). Head position and parsing ambiguity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 201, 251–270.
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL]
Sanoudaki, E., & Thierry, G. (2015). Language non-selective syntactic activation in early bilinguals: the role of verbal fluency. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 181, 548–560.
Seo, M. (2018). A study on the syntax of Korean double object. Emwunyenkwu, 981, 5–26.
Shibatani, M. (1990). The Languages of Japan. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Shibatani, M., & Pardeshi, P. (2002). The causative continuum. Typological studies in language, 481, 85–126.
Sohn, H. M. (2001). The Korean Language. Cambridge University Press.
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar. An event-related potential
investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 271, 173–204.
Tolentino, L. C., & Tokowicz, N. (2011). Across languages, space and time: A review of the role of cross-language similarity in L2 (morpho)syntactic
processing as revealed by fMRI and ERP methods. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 91–125.
Trenkic, D., Mirkovic, J., & Altmann, G. T. (2014). Real-time grammar processing by native and non-native speakers: Constructions unique to the second
language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 171, 237–257.
van Hell, J. G., & Tokowicz, N. (2010). Event-related brain potentials and second language learning: Syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different
L2 proficiency levels. Second Language Research, 261, 43–74.
Wolff, P., & Song, G. (2003). Models of causation and the semantics of causal verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 471, 276–332.
2024. L2 textbook input and L2 written production: a case of Korean locative postposition–verb construction. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 62:2 ► pp. 539 ff.
2024. Good-enough processing, home language proficiency, cognitive skills, and task effects for Korean heritage speakers’ sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 15
Lee, Chanyoung, Gyu-Ho Shin & Boo Kyung Jung
2023. How ‘good-enough’ is second language comprehension? Morphological causative and suffixal passive constructions in Korean. Applied Linguistics Review
Hao, Yuxin, Xun Duan & Qiuyue Yan
2022. Processing Aspectual Agreement in a Language with Limited Morphological Inflection by Second Language Learners: An ERP Study of Mandarin Chinese. Brain Sciences 12:5 ► pp. 524 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.