Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 13:4 (2023) ► pp.468499
References
Abbot-Smith, K., Chang, F., Rowland, C., Ferguson, H., & Pine, J.
(2017) Do two and three year old children use an incremental first-NP-as-agent bias to process active transitive and passive sentences?: A permutation analysis. PLOS ONE, 12 (10), 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aizu, Y.
(2016) Acquisition and Processing of Japanese Passives by Heritage Speakers and JFL Learners: Effects of Manner of Input and Early Age of Acquisition [Doctorial Dissertation]. University of Ottawa.
Argyri, E., & Sorace, A.
(2007) Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10 (1), 79–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Walters, J., & Gagarina, N.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68 (3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bosch, J. E., & Unsworth, S.
(2020) Cross-linguistic influence in word order: Effects of age, dominance and surface overlap. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J.
(2017) An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40 1, e282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M.
(1999) Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Developmental Psychology, 35 (1), 29–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chan, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Children’s understanding of the agent-patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Schwartz, R. G.
(2020) Case marking and word order in Greek heritage children. Journal of Child Language, 1–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daskalaki, E., Chondrogianni, V., Blom, E., Argyri, F., & Paradis, J.
(2019) Input effects across domains: The case of Greek subjects in child heritage language. Second Language Research, 35 (3), 421–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K.
(1989) Maturation and the Acquisition of the Sesotho Passive. Language, 65 (1), 56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deng, X., Mai, Z., & Yip, V.
(2018) An aspectual account of ba and bei constructions in child Mandarin. First Language, 38 (3), 243–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2008) German Children’s Comprehension of Word Order and Case Marking in Causative Sentences. Child Development, 79 (4), 1152–1167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flores, C., & Barbosa, P.
(2014) When reduced input leads to delayed acquisition: A study on the acquisition of clitic placement by Portuguese heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18 (3), 304–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flores, C., Santos, A. L., Jesus, A., & Marques, R.
(2017) Age and input effects in the acquisition of mood in Heritage Portuguese. Journal of Child Language, 44 (4), 795–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gagarina, N., & Klassert, A.
(2018) Input Dominance and Development of Home Language in Russian-German Bilinguals. Frontiers in Communication, 3 1, Article 40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J., Li, Y. A., & Li, Y.
(2009) The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y. T., Zheng, X., Meng, X., & Snedeker, J.
(2013) Children’s assignment of grammatical roles in the online processing of Mandarin passive sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 69 (4), 589–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janssen, B.
(2016) The acquisition of gender and case in Polish and Russian: A study of monolingual and bilingual children [PhD Dissertation]. University of Amsterdam.
Jia, R., & Paradis, J.
(2018) The acquisition of relative clauses by Mandarin heritage language children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10(2), 153–183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Chan, A., & Chiu, J.
(2015) Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous Cantonese–English bilingual children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (3), 438–452. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, K., O’Grady, W., & Schwartz, B. D.
(2018) Case in Heritage Korean. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8 (2), 252–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Bates, E., Liu, H., & MacWhinney, B.
(1992) Cues as Functional Constraints on Sentence Processing in Chinese. In H.-C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng, Language Processing in Chinese. Advances in Psychology, Vol. 901 (pp. 207–234). Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mai, Z., Kwan, C., & Yip, V.
(2018) Expressing displacement in heritage Cantonese: Cross-linguistic influence and structural vulnerability. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22 (6), 603–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A.
(2012) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 (4), 568–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., & Hulk, A.
(2001) Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (1), 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Omaki, A., Davidson White, I., Goro, T., Lidz, J., & Phillips, C.
(2014) No Fear of Commitment: Children’s Incremental Interpretation in English and Japanese Wh-Questions. Language Learning and Development, 10 (3), 206–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M.
(2008) Gender Under Incomplete Acquisition: Heritage Speakers’ Knowledge of Noun Categorization. Heritage Language Journal, 6 (1), 40–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G.
(2020) Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23 (1), 4–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., Zhang, B., & Gallo, C. G.
(2010) Heritage Chinese: A New View From Production. Heritage Language Insititute IV, University of California.Google Scholar
R Core Team
(2018) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [URL]
Serratrice, L.
(2016) Cross-linguistic influence, cross-linguistic priming and the nature of shared syntactic structures. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6 (6), 822–827. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S.
(2004) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax–pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English–Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7 (3), 183–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R.
(2015) The effect of construction frequency and native transfer on second language knowledge of the syntax–discourse interface. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36 (3), 671–699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, E. M., Williams, N., Jones, L. A., Davies, S., & Binks, H.
(2014) Acquiring complex structures under minority language conditions: Bilingual acquisition of plural morphology in Welsh. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (3), 478–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2000) Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74 (3), 209–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., & Gómez, L.
(2012) Using priming procedures with children. In E. Hoff (Ed.), Research Methods in Child Language (pp. 162–176). Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhou, P., & Ma, W.
(2018) Children’s Use of Morphological Cues in Real-Time Event Representation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 47 (1), 241–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Chondrogianni, Vicky
2023. Chapter 12. Cross-linguistic influences in bilingual morphosyntactic acquisition. In Cross-language Influences in Bilingual Processing and Second Language Acquisition [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 16],  pp. 294 ff. DOI logo
CHONDROGIANNI, Vicky
2023. Individual differences differentially influence language domains and learning mechanisms. Journal of Child Language  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Ji, Yue, Li Sheng & Li Zheng
2023. Acquisition of non-canonical word orders in Mandarin Chinese. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 1 may 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.