Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 13:3 (2023) ► pp.372408
References
Anthony, J. L. & Francis, D. J.
(2005) Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 (5), 255–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., Escudero, P., & Hayes-Harb, R.
(2015) Second language phonology at the interface between acoustic and orthographic input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36 (1), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 (1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D.
(2007) Non-native and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In O. -S. Bohn & M. Munro. (Eds.), Language experience in second-language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 13–34). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bieber, R. E., & Gordon-Salant, S.
(2021) Improving older adults’ understanding of challenging speech: Auditory training, rapid adaptation and perceptual learning. Hearing Research, 402 1, 108054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broersma, M.
(2010) Perception of final fricative voicing: native and nonnative listeners’ use of vowel duration. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127 (3), 1636–1644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Increased lexical activation and reduced competition in second-language listening. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27 (7–8), 1205–1224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broersma, M., & Cutler, A.
(2008) Phantom word activation in L2. System, 36 (1), 22–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, A.
(1995) Minimal pairs: Minimal importance? ELT Journal, 49 (2), 169–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Booij, G.
(1999) The phonology of Dutch. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & New, B.
(2009) Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41 1, 977–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chandrasekaran, B., Yi, H., Smayda, K. E., & Maddox, W. T.
(2016) Effect of explicit dimensional instruction on speech category learning. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78 1, 566–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, Y., & Pederson, E.
(2017) Directing attention during perceptual training: A preliminary study of phonetic learning in Southern Min by Mandarin speakers. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2017 (pp. 1770–1774). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, B., & Mees, I. M.
(1996) The phonetics of English and Dutch (3rd ed.). E. J. Brill.Google Scholar
Díaz, B., Mitterer, H., Broersma, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2012) Individual differences in late bilinguals’ L2 phonological processes: From acoustic-phonetic analysis to lexical access. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(6), 680–689. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eisner, F. & McQueen, J. M.
(2005) The specificity of perceptual learning in speech processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 67 (2), 224–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Field, J.
(2008) Listening in the language classroom. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. & Bohn, O. -S.
(2021) The revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In R. Wayland. (Ed.), Second language speech learning: Theoretical and empirical progress (pp. 3–83). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Francis, A. L., & Nusbaum, H. C.
(2002) Selective attention and the acquisition of new phonetic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28 (2), 349–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guion, S. G., & Pederson, E.
(2007) Investigating the role of attention in phonetic learning. In O. S. Bohn & M. Munro. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning (pp. 57–77). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon-Salant, S.
(2005) Hearing loss and aging: New research findings and clinical implications. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 42 (4), 9–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hisagi, M., & Strange, W.
(2011) Perception of Japanese temporally-cued contrasts by American English listeners. Language and Speech, 54 (2), 241–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
House, A. S.
(1961) On vowel duration in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33 (9), 1174–1178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hillenbrand, J. M., Getty, L. A., Clark, M. J., & Wheeler, K.
(1995) Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97 (5), 3099–3111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janssen, C., Segers, E., McQueen, J. M., & Verhoeven, L.
(2015) Lexical specificity training effects in second language learners. Language Learning, 65 (2), 358–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kissling, E. M.
(2014) Phonetics instruction improves learners’ perception of L2 sounds. Language Teaching Research, 19 (3), 254–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krenca, K., Segers, E., Chen, X., Shakory, S., Steele, J., & Verhoeven, L.
(2020) Phonological specificity relates to phonological awareness and reading ability in English-French bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 33 1, 267–291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kubo, R., & Akahane-Yamada, R.
(2006) Influence of aging on perceptual learning of English phonetic contrasts by native speakers of Japanese. Acoustical Science and Technology, 27 (1), 59–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maddox, W. T., Chandrasekaran, B., Smayda, K., & Yi, H.
(2013) Dual systems of speech category learning across the lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 28 (4), 1042–1056. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pederson, E., & Guion-Anderson, S.
(2010) Orienting attention during phonetic training facilitates learning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127 (2), EL54–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peelle, J. E., & Wingfield, A.
(2005) Dissociations in perceptual learning revealed by adult age differences in adaptation to time-compressed speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31 (6), 1315–1330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pichora-Fuller, M. K., & Souza, P. E.
(2003) Effects of aging on auditory processing of speech. International Journal of Audiology, 42 (Suppl2), 2S11–2S16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Porretta, V. J., & Tucker, B. V.
(2014) Perception of non-native consonant length contrast: The role of attention in phonetic processing. Second Language Research, 31 (2), 239–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raphael, L. J.
(2005) Acoustic cues to the perception of segmental phonemes. In D. B. Pisoni & R. E. Remez. (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 182–206). Blackwell Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sakai, M., & Moorman, C.
(2018) Can perception training improve the production of second language phonemes? A meta-analytic review of 25 years of perception training research. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39 1, 187–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schepens, J., Dijkstra, T., & Grootjen, F.
(2012) Distributions of cognates in Europe as based on Levenshtein distance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (1), 157–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scharenbourg, O., & Janse, E.
(2013) Comparing lexically guided perceptual learning in younger and older listeners. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75 1, 525–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slis, I. H., & Cohen, A.
(1969) On the complex regulating the voiced-voiceless distinction I. Language and Speech, 12 (2), 80–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sommers, M. S.
(1997) Stimulus variability and spoken word recognition. II. The effects of age and hearing impairment. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101 (4), 2278–2288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Svalberg, A. M. L.
(2007) Language awareness and language learning. Language Teaching, 40 (4), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, R. S., & Villa, V.
(1994) Attention in cognitive science and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16 1, 183–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van der Feest, S. V. H., & Swingley, D.
(2011) Dutch and English listeners’ interpretation of vowel duration. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129 (3), EL57–EL63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Leussen, J. -W., & Escudero, P.
(2015) Learning to perceive and recognize a second language: The L2LP model revised. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 1, 1000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar