Commentary published In:
Epistemological issue: Bilingual Language Development in Autism
Edited by Cristina Flores and Neal Snape
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 12:1] 2022
► pp. 7681
References (15)
References
Constantino, J. N. (2011). The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatratic Research, 69 1, 55–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haberzettl, S. (2005). Der Erwerb der Verbstellungsregeln in der Zweitsprache Deutsch durch Kinder mit russischer und türkischer Muttersprache (Reihe Linguistische Arbeiten). Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. (2009). Second language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 28 (1), 5–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mottron, L., & Bzdok, D. (2020). Autism spectrum heterogeneity: fact or artifact? Molecular Psychiatry, 25 1, 3178–3185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mottron, L., Ostrolenk, A., & Gagnon, D. (2021). In prototypical autism, the genetic ability to learn language is triggered by structured information, not only by exposure to oral language. Genes, 12 1, 1112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nunes, A., Trappenberg, T., & Alda, M. (2020). The definition and measurement of heterogeneity. Translational Psychiatry, 10 1, Article 299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Novogrodsky, R. (2015). Specific language impairment (SLI) is not specific enough: Sub-types of SLI and their implication for the theory of disorder. In A. Stavrakaki. (Ed.), Specific Language Impairment. Current Trends in Research. [ Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 58] (pp. 113–123). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & Tuller, L. (2022). Bilingual language development in autism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P., & Friedmann, N. (2011). Specific Language Impairment (SLI) across languages: Properties and possible loci. Lingua, 121 (3), 333–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sukenik, N., Morin, E., Friedmann, N., Prévost, P., & Tuller, L. (2021). Coconuts and curtain cakes: The production of wh-questions in ASD. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 6 1, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P., & Tracy, R. (2018). Revisiting the tolerance of Universal Grammar. In B. Hollebrandse, J. Kim, J., A. T. Pérez-Leroux & P. Schulz. (Eds.), T.O.M. and Grammar. Thoughts on Mind and Grammar: A Festschrift in honor of Tom Roeper. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics (UMOP) 411 (pp. 129–145). University of Massachusetts, Graduate Linguistics Student Association.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Grimm, A. (2019). The age factor revisited: Timing in acquisition interacts with age of onset in bilingual acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 1, Article 2732. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for Conducting Research on Language in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34 (1), 75–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tracy, R., & Thoma, D. (2009). Convergence on finite V2 clauses in L1, bilingual L1 and early L2 acquisition. In C. Dimroth & P. Jordens. (Eds.), Functional categories in learner language. Studies on Language Acquisition (SOLA) (pp. 1–43). de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2014). Early, late or very late? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4 1, 283–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Andreou, Maria & Vasileia Skrimpa
2022. Re-Examining Labels in Neurocognitive Research: Evidence from Bilingualism and Autism as Spectrum-Trait Cases. Brain Sciences 12:8  pp. 1113 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.