References (75)
References
Alonso, M. A., Fernandez, A., & Díez, E. (2015). Subjective age-of-acquisition norms for 7,039 Spanish words. Behavior Research Methods, 47 (1), 268–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baguley, T. S. (2012). Serious stats: A guide to advanced statistics for the behavioural sciences. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: the effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19 (3), 241–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67 (1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (5), 931–949.Google Scholar
(2013). From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition, 127 (3), 287–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K. J. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18 1, 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K. J., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of emphasis in language production: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129 1, 177–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Jones, M. (2005). A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., De Deyne, S., Voorspoels, W., & Storms, G. (2014). Norms of age of acquisition and concreteness for 30,000 Dutch words. Acta Psychologica, 150 1, 80–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., Yan, H., & Branigan, H. P. (2011). Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65 (4), 431–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F. (2002). Symbolically speaking: A connectionist model of sentence production. Cognitive Science, 26 1, 609–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113 (2), 234–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (3), 175–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10 (1), 101–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics (4th edition). SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Flett, S., Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). Are non-native structural preferences affected by native language preferences? Bilingualism, 16 (4), 751–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22 (3), 611–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodhew, S. C., & Edwards, M. (2019). Translating experimental paradigms into individual-differences research: Contributions, challenges, and practical recommendations. Consciousness and Cognition, 69 1(November 2018), 14–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., Baum, S., Klein, D., & Titone, D. (2018). Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia, 117 1, 123–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haeseryn, W., Romijn, K., Geerts, G., de Rooij, J., & van den Toorn, M. C. (1997). Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst. Martinus Nijhoff/Wolters Plantyn.Google Scholar
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., Polišenská, K., Mieszkowska, K., & Chiat, S. (2013). Cross-linguistic Lexical Tasks: British English version (CLT-EN). As part of LITMUS COST IS0804 Battery. Unpublished material.Google Scholar
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., & Pomiechowska, B. (2015). Designing Cross-linguistic Lexical Tasks (CLTs) for bilingual preschool children. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. De Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 196–240). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S. (2017). The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (2), 219–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2008). Language integration in bilingual sentence production. Acta Psychologica, 128 1, 479–489. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15 1, 409–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hervé, C., Serratrice, L., & Corley, M. (2016). Dislocations in French–English bilingual children: An elicitation study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (05), 987–1000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsin, L., Legendre, G., & Omaki, A. (2013). Priming cross-linguistic interference in bilingual children. BUCLD 37: Proceedings of the 37th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, September 2016, 165–177.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. N., & Ruf, H. T. (2017). The priming of word order in second language German. In Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E. (2013). Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127 (1), 57–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., & Chun, E. (2018). Priming and adaptation in native speakers and second-language learners. Bilingualism, 21 (2), 228–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kantola, L., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2011). Between- and within-language priming is the same: evidence for shared bilingual syntactic representations. Memory & Cognition, 39 (2), 276–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Coyle, J. M. (2015). Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (6), 728–743. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E. (2012). Individual differences in syntactic priming in language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 (02), 393–418. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kootstra, G. J., & Doedens, W. J. (2016). How multiple sources of experience influence bilingual syntactic choice: Immediate and cumulative cross-languaage effects of structural priming, verb bias and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (4), 710–732. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kootstra, G. J., van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2010). Syntactic alignment and shared word order in code-switched sentence production: Evidence from bilingual monologue and dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (2), 210–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30 thousand English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44 (4), 978–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lanza, E. (1992). Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language, 19 1, 633–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemmerth, N., & Hopp, H. (2019). Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic influences. Language Acquisition, 26 (1), 21–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22 1, 1–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D. (2021). sjPlot: Data visualization for statistics in social science. (R package). [URL]
Meisel, J. M. (2007). On autonomous syntactic development in multiple first language acquisition. In H. Caunt-Nulton, S. Kulatilake, & I.-H. Woo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Boston University conference on language development (pp. 26–45).Google Scholar
Messenger, K. (2021). The Persistence of Priming: Exploring Long-lasting Syntactic Priming Effects in Children and Adults. Cognitive Science, 45 (6). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montero-Melis, G., & Jaeger, T. F. (2020). Changing expectations mediate adaptation in L2 production. Bilingualism, 23 (3), 602–617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muylle, M., Bernolet, S., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2021). The role of L1 and L2 frequency in cross-linguistic structural priming: An artificial language learning study. Bilingualism, 24 (4), 767–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myers-Scotton, C. (1993). Social motivations for codeswitching: Evidence from Africa. Carendon Press.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2006). Cross-linguistic transfer in adjective–noun strings by preschool bilingual children. Bilingualism, 9 (01), 15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Cross-linguistic influence in French–English bilingual children’s possessive constructions. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (02), 320–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwenhuis, R., te Grotenhuis, M., & Pelzer, B. (2012). Influence.ME: Tools for detecting influential data in mixed effects models. R Journal, 4 (2), 38–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Connor, C., Maling, J., & Skarabela, B. (2013). Nominal categories and the expression of possession. In K. Börjars, D. Denison, & A. Scott (Eds.), Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession (pp. 89–121). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic Acquisition in Bilingual Children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18 (1), 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39 1, 633–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reitter, D., Keller, F., & Moore, J. D. (2011). A Computational Cognitive Model of Syntactic Priming. Cognitive Science, 35 (4), 587–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenbach, A. (2014). English genitive variation – the state of the art. English Language and Linguistics, 18 (2), 215–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125 (1), 49–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Runnqvist, E., Gollan, T. H., Costa, A., & Ferreira, V. S. (2013). A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages. Cognition, 129 1, 256–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56 1, 153–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2013). Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual development. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3 (1), 3–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Cross-linguistic influence, cross-linguistic priming and the nature of shared syntactic structures. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6 (6), 822–827. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022). What can syntactic priming tell us about crosslinguistic influence? In K. Messenger (Ed.), Syntactic priming in language acquisition: Representations, mechanisms and applications (pp. 129–156). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Hesketh, A., & Ashworth, R. (2015). The use of reported speech in children’s narratives: A priming study. First Language, 35 (1), 68–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B., & Serratrice, L. (2009). “The doctor’s mother” or “the mother of the doctor”?: Syntactic priming of possessive noun phrases in English preschoolers. In J. Chandlee, M. Fanchini, S. Lord, & M. Rheiner (Eds.), BUCLD 33 Online Proceedings Suppplement. Cascadilla Press. [URL]
Snedeker, J., & Huang, Y. T. (2015). Sentence processing. In E. L. Bavin & R. Naigles Letitia (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 409–437). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Team, Rs. (2020). RStudio: Integrated development for R. RStudio, PBC.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism, 16 (1), 86–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S., Chondrogianni, V., & Skarabela, B. (2018). Experiential Measures Can Be Used as a Proxy for Language Dominance in Bilingual Language Acquisition Research. In Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G. (2011). Who’s first and what’s next? Animacy and word order variation in Dutch language production. Radboud University, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
van der Linden, E., & Blok-Boas, A. (2005). Exploring possession in simultaneous bilingualism: Dutch/French and Dutch/Italian. EUROSLA Yearbook, 5 (1), 103–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, C., Dijkstra, T., & Unsworth, S. (2022). Cross-linguistic influence during online sentence processing in bilingual children. Bilingualism, 25 (4), 691–704. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, C. N., van Wonderen, E., Koutamanis, E., Kootstra, G. J., Dijkstra, T., & Unsworth, S. (2021). Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children: a meta-analysis. Journal of Child Language. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P. G., & Arai, M. (2018). Structural priming in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21 (3), 448–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Kampen, J., & Corver, N. (2006). Diversity of possessor marking in Dutch child language and Dutch dialects. In M. Vliegen (Ed.), Variation in Sprachtheorie und Spracherwerb (pp. 385–398). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
van Wonderen, E., & Unsworth, S. (2020). Testing the validity of the Cross-Linguistic Lexical Task as a measure of language proficiency in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E. P., & Shimpi, P. M. (2010). Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37 (05), 1047–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolleb, A., Sorace, A., & Westergaard, M. (2018). Exploring the role of cognitive control in syntactic processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8 (5), 606–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (5)

Cited by five other publications

Barking, Marie, Maria Mos & Ad Backus
Koutamanis, Elly, Gerrit Jan Kootstra, Ton Dijkstra & Sharon Unsworth
2024. The Role of Cognates and Language Distance in Simultaneous Bilingual Children's Productive Vocabulary Acquisition. Language Learning DOI logo
Pontikas, George, Ian Cunnings & Theodoros Marinis
2024. Sentence processing in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 476 ff. DOI logo
van Dijk, Chantal, Jasmijn Bosch & Sharon Unsworth
2024. Language processing in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 435 ff. DOI logo
Xu, Kexin, Tao Zeng & Claudia Felser
2024. Cross-linguistic syntactic priming as rational expectation for syntactic repetition in the bilingual environment. PLOS ONE 19:7  pp. e0307504 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.