Listeners use linguistic cues to anticipate upcoming words, but not all do so to the same extent. While we know
that monolingual children use prediction during spoken language processing from a young age and that adult L2 speakers may
sometimes be slower, very few studies have focused on bilingual or multilingual children. Moreover, previous research suggests
that literacy boosts anticipation in spoken language processing, but this has not been tested yet in bi/multilinguals. We
investigated linguistic prediction and its relation to reading and vocabulary skills in 38 eight- to twelve-year-old bilingual and
multilingual children who speak different heritage languages and Italian as the majority language, in comparison to 32 age-matched
monolingual Italian children. Using a visual world eye tracking method, we tested children’s ability to anticipate nouns based on
morphosyntactic cues (gender- and number-marked articles) in Italian. The results show efficient prediction in both groups,
although monolinguals were faster than bi/multilinguals. While we found a positive relation between predictive language processing
and reading in monolingual children, there were no reliable effects in bilingual and multilingual children. Future work is
required to better understand the relation between prediction and literacy in this population.
Altmann, G. T. (2011). The
mediation of eye movements by spoken language. In S. Liversedge, I. Gilchrist & S. Everling (Eds.), The
Oxford Handbook of Eye
Movements (pp. 979–1004). Oxford University Press.
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental
interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent
reference. Cognition,
73
(3), 247–264.
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The
real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to
linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and
Language,
57
(4), 502–518.
Andrews, S. (2008). Lexical
expertise and reading skill. In K. D. Federmeier & J. L. Montag (Eds.), The
Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Speaking, Writing and
Communicating (pp. 247–281). Elsevier Academic Press.
Antón-Méndez, I., Nicol, J. L., & Garrett, M. F. (2002). The
relation between gender and number agreement
processing. Syntax,
5
(1), 1–25.
Audacity
Team (2020). Audacity(R): Free Audio Editor and
Recorder [Computer application].
Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical
gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience,
17
(1), 137–153.
Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker & S. Walker. (2015). Fitting
Linear Mixed Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical
Software, 67(1), 1–48.
Bianchi, G. (2013). Gender
in Italian–German bilinguals: A comparison with German L2 learners of Italian. Bilingualism:
Language and
Cognition, 16(3), 538–557.
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed
of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental
science, 14(2), 256–269.
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing
a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in
children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology,
112
(4), 417–436.
Bosch, J. E. & Foppolo, F. (2022). Predictive
processing of grammatical gender in bilingual children: The effect of cross-linguistic incongruency and language
dominance. Lingue e
Linguaggio,
21
(1), 5–27.
Brouwer, S., Özkan, D., & Küntay, A. C. (2017). Semantic
prediction in monolingual and bilingual children. In Blom, E., Cornips, L. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Cross-linguistic
influence in Bilingualism: In honor of Aafke
Hulk (pp. 49–74). John Benjamins Publishing.
Caprin, C., & Guasti, M. T. (2009). The
acquisition of morphosyntax in Italian: A cross-sectional study. Applied
psycholinguistics,
30
(1), 23–52.
Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., & Stefanini, S. (2007). Parole
e frasi nel «Primo vocabolario del bambino». Nuovi dati normativi fra i 18 e 36 mesi e forma breve del
questionario. FrancoAngeli.
Chun, E., & Kaan, E. (2019). L2
Prediction during complex sentence processing. Journal of Cultural Cognitive
Science,
3
(2), 203–216.
Cipriani, P., A. M. Chiosi, P. Bottari & L. Pfanner. (1993). L’acquisizione
della morfosintassi in italiano: Fasi e
processi. Unipress.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical
processing in language learners. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
27
(1), 3–42.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2018). Some
notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition,
40
(3), 693–706.
Cornoldi, C. & Carretti, B. (2016). Prove
di lettura MT-3-Clinica; la valutazione delle abilità di lettura e comprensione. Giunti EDU.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What
reading does for the mind. American
Educator,
22
(1–2), 8–17.
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading
in the brain: The new science of how we
read. Penguin.
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J. & Cohen, L. (2010). How
learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and
language. Science,
330
(6009), 1359–1364.
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic
word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain
activity. Nature
Neuroscience,
8
(8), 1117–1121.
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2017). Predicting
upcoming information in native language and non-native-language auditory word
recognition. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
20
(5), 917–930.
Dispaldro, M., Ruggiero, A., & Scali, F. (2015). Real-time
comprehension of gender and number in four-to seven-year-old children: a study of the relationship between Italian clitic
pronouns and visual picture referents. Journal of Child
Language,
42
(4), 917–931.
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When
gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in
Second Language
Acquisition,
35
(2), 353–387.
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic
mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary
learning. Scientific Studies of
Reading,
18
(1), 5–21.
Favier, S., Meyer, A., & Huettig, F. (2021). Literacy
can enhance syntactic prediction in spoken language processing. Journal of Experimental
Psychology:
General,
150
(10), 2167–2174.
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A
rose by any other name: Long–term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory
and
Language,
41
(4), 469–495.
Federmeier, K. D., Kutas, M., & Schul, R. (2010). Age-related
and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and
Language,
115
(3), 149–161.
Ferrari, A. & Zampese, L. (2016). Grammatica:
parole, frasi, testi dell’italiano. Carocci.
Foppolo, F., & Marelli, M. (2017). No
delay for some inferences. Journal of
Semantics,
34
(4), 659–681.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical
gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1–L2 syntactic
similarity. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
14
(3), 379–399.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Can
late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal
of Memory and
Language,
66
(1), 226–248.
Gillon-Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A. & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic
processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience,
22
(8), 1870–1887.
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical
gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem?. Second Language
Research,
28
(2), 191–215.
Guasti, M. T., Pagliarini, E., & Stucchi, N. (2017). Language,
Reading, and Motor Control: Get
Rhythm!. Biolinguistics,
11
1, 395–406.
Havron, N., de Carvalho, A., Fiévet, A. C., & Christophe, A. (2019). Three-to
four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word
meanings. Child
Development,
90
(1), 82–90.
Hopp, H. (2015). Semantics
and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied
Linguistics in Language
Teaching,
53
(3), 277–306.
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N. (2018). Lexical
and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition,
40
(1), 171–199.
Huettig, F., & Brouwer, S. (2015). Delayed
anticipatory spoken language processing in adults with dyslexia – evidence from
eye-tracking. Dyslexia,
21
(2), 97–122.
Huettig, F., Lachmann, T., Reis, A., & Petersson, K. M. (2018). Distinguishing
cause from effect – many deficits associated with developmental dyslexia may be a consequence of reduced and suboptimal
reading experience. Language, Cognition and
Neuroscience,
33
(3), 333–350.
Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is
prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition and
Neuroscience,
31
(1), 19–31.
Huettig, F., & Pickering, M. J. (2019). Literacy
advantages beyond reading: Prediction of spoken language. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences,
23
(6), 464–475.
Igoa, J. M., García-Albea, J. E., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (1999). Gender-number
dissociations in sentence production in Spanish. Rivista di
Linguistica,
11
(1), 163–196.
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J. (2018). A
cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2
comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
21
(2), 251–264.
Kouider, S., Halberda, J., Wood, J., & Carey, S. (2006). Acquisition
of English number marking: The singular-plural distinction. Language Learning and
Development,
2
(1), 1–25.
Ladas, A. I., D. J. Carroll & A. B. Vivas. (2015). Attentional
processes in low socioeconomic status bilingual children: Are they modulated by the amount of bilingual
experience?. Child
Development,
86
(2), 557–578.
Lê, S., Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR:
an R package for multivariate analysis. Journal of Statistical
Software,
25
(1), 1–18.
Lemmerth, N., & Hopp, H. (2019). Gender
processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic
influences. Language
Acquisition,
26
(1), 21–45.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young
children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word
recognition. Psychological
Science,
18
(3), 193–198.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time
processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of
Memory and
Language,
63
(4), 447–464.
Mani, N., & Huettig, F. (2012). Prediction
during language processing is a piece of cake – But only for skilled producers. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance,
38
(4), 843–847.
Mani, N., & Huettig, F. (2014). Word
reading skill predicts anticipation of upcoming spoken language input: A study of children developing proficiency in
reading. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology,
126
1, 264–279.
Marini, A., Marotta, L., Bulgheroni, S. & Fabbro, F. (2015). Batteria
per la valutazione del linguaggio in bambini dai 4 ai 12 anni (BVL_4–12). Giunti O.S.
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals
reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of
Memory and
Language,
69
(4), 574–588.
Meir, N., Parshina, O., & Sekerina, I. A. (2020). The
interaction of morphological cues in bilingual sentence processing: an eye-tracking
study. In M. M. Brown & A. Kohut, BUCLD44:
Proceedings of the 44th Annual Boston University Conference on Language
Development (pp. 376–389). Cascadilla Press.
Meisel, J. M. (2018). Early
child second language acquisition: French gender in German children. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
21
(4), 656–673.
Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F. (2012). Spoken
language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high
literates. Journal of Eye Movement
Research,
5
(1), 1–10.
Mitsugi, S., & Macwhinney, B. (2016). The
use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism:
Language and
Cognition,
19
(1), 19–35.
Morales, L., Paolieri, D., Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Gerfen, C., & Bajo, M. T. (2016). The
gender congruency effect during bilingual spoken-word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition,
19
(2), 294–310.
Mornati, G., Riva, V., Vismara, E., Molteni, M., & Cantiani, C. (2022). Infants
aged 12 months use the gender feature in determiners to anticipate upcoming words: an eye-tracking
study. Journal of Child
Language,
50
(4), 1–19.
Ng, S., Payne, B. R., Stine-Morrow, E. A., & Federmeier, K. D. (2018). How
struggling adult readers use contextual information when comprehending speech: Evidence from event related
potentials. International Journal of
Psychophysiology,
125
1, 1–9.
Pagliarini, E., Scocchia, L., Granocchio, E., Sarti, D., Stucchi, N., & Guasti, M. T. (2020). Timing
anticipation in adults and children with Developmental Dyslexia: evidence of an inefficient
mechanism. Scientific
Reports,
10
(1), 1–15.
Panzini, A. (2017). Grammatica
italiana. Edizioni Lindau.
Persici, V., Stucchi, N., & Arosio, F. (2019). Rhythmic
and Morphosyntactic Predictions: The Anticipation Abilities of Italian Children with Developmental
Dyslexia. In M. M. Brown & B. Dailey, BUCLD43:
Proceedings of the 43rd annual Boston University Conference on Language
Development (pp. 537–548). Cascadilla Press.
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting
while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological
Bulletin,
144
(10), 1002–1044.
Psychology Software Tools,
Inc. (2020). E-Prime 3.0.
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and
environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The
EZ Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and
Brain
Sciences,
26
(4), 445–476.
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Verbal
and nonverbal predictors of language mediated anticipatory eye movements. Attention, Perception
&
Psychophysics,
77
(3), 720–730.
Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2007). DDE-2.
Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva. Giunti OS.
Segalowitz, N. & Hulstijn, J. (2005). Automaticity
in bilingualism and second language learning. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook
of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic
Approaches (pp. 371–388). Oxford University Press.
Staub, A., & Clifton Jr, C. (2006). Syntactic
prediction in language comprehension: evidence from either... or. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition,
32
(2), 425–436.
Tsimpli, I. M., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The
interpretability hypothesis: Evidence from wh- interrogatives in second language
acquisition. Second Language
Research,
23
(2), 215–242.
Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A., & Tsimpli, I. (2014). The
role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
35
(4), 765–805.
Van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating
upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition,
31
(3), 443–467.
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska–Macgregor, M. & Leung, Y. K. I. (2004). Gender
and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
25
(1), 105–133.
Cited by (5)
Cited by five other publications
Baron, Alisa, Katrina Connell, Daniel Kleinman, Lisa M. Bedore & Zenzi M. Griffin
2024. Grammatical gender in spoken word recognition in school-age Spanish monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual children. Frontiers in Psychology 15
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.