Article published In:
Processing in Bilingual Children
Edited by Chantal van Dijk, Jasmijn E. Bosch and Sharon Unsworth
[Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4] 2024
► pp. 512543
References (89)
References
Altmann, G. T. (2011). The mediation of eye movements by spoken language. In S. Liversedge, I. Gilchrist & S. Everling (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Eye Movements (pp. 979–1004). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73 (3), 247–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57 (4), 502–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S. (2008). Lexical expertise and reading skill. In K. D. Federmeier & J. L. Montag (Eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Speaking, Writing and Communicating (pp. 247–281). Elsevier Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antón-Méndez, I., Nicol, J. L., & Garrett, M. F. (2002). The relation between gender and number agreement processing. Syntax, 5 (1), 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Audacity Team (2020). Audacity(R): Free Audio Editor and Recorder [Computer application].Google Scholar
Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 (1), 137–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker & S. Walker. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bianchi, G. (2013). Gender in Italian–German bilinguals: A comparison with German L2 learners of Italian. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(3), 538–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental science, 14(2), 256–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112 (4), 417–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bosch, J. E., Chailleux, M. A., Yee, J., Guasti, M. T., & Arosio, F. (2022). Prediction on the basis of gender and number in Mandarin-Italian bilingual children. In Ayoun, D. (Ed.), The expression and acquisition of gender (pp. 243–271). John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bosch, J. E. & Foppolo, F. (2022). Predictive processing of grammatical gender in bilingual children: The effect of cross-linguistic incongruency and language dominance. Lingue e Linguaggio, 21 (1), 5–27.Google Scholar
Bosch, J. E., & Unsworth, S. (2021). Cross-linguistic influence in word order: Effects of age, dominance and surface overlap. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11 (6), 783–816. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., Özkan, D., & Küntay, A. C. (2017). Semantic prediction in monolingual and bilingual children. In Blom, E., Cornips, L. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in Bilingualism: In honor of Aafke Hulk (pp. 49–74). John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caprin, C., & Guasti, M. T. (2009). The acquisition of morphosyntax in Italian: A cross-sectional study. Applied psycholinguistics, 30 (1), 23–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., & Stefanini, S. (2007). Parole e frasi nel «Primo vocabolario del bambino». Nuovi dati normativi fra i 18 e 36 mesi e forma breve del questionario. FrancoAngeli.Google Scholar
Chun, E., & Kaan, E. (2019). L2 Prediction during complex sentence processing. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3 (2), 203–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cipriani, P., A. M. Chiosi, P. Bottari & L. Pfanner. (1993). L’acquisizione della morfosintassi in italiano: Fasi e processi. Unipress.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27 (1), 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40 (3), 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cornoldi, C. & Carretti, B. (2016). Prove di lettura MT-3-Clinica; la valutazione delle abilità di lettura e comprensione. Giunti EDU.Google Scholar
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1998). What reading does for the mind. American Educator, 22 (1–2), 8–17.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Penguin.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J. & Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330 (6009), 1359–1364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8 (8), 1117–1121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2017). Predicting upcoming information in native language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20 (5), 917–930. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dispaldro, M., Ruggiero, A., & Scali, F. (2015). Real-time comprehension of gender and number in four-to seven-year-old children: a study of the relationship between Italian clitic pronouns and visual picture referents. Journal of Child Language, 42 (4), 917–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 (2), 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18 (1), 5–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Favier, S., Meyer, A., & Huettig, F. (2021). Literacy can enhance syntactic prediction in spoken language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150 (10), 2167–2174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long–term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41 (4), 469–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., Kutas, M., & Schul, R. (2010). Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and Language, 115 (3), 149–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, A. & Zampese, L. (2016). Grammatica: parole, frasi, testi dell’italiano. Carocci.Google Scholar
Foppolo, F., & Marelli, M. (2017). No delay for some inferences. Journal of Semantics, 34 (4), 659–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1–L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 379–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 (1), 226–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, Z. (2022). Facilitative use of grammatical gender in Heritage Spanish. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12 (6), 845–871. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gabriele, A., Fiorentino, R. & Bañón, J. A. (2013). Examining second language development using event-related potentials: a cross-sectional study on the processing of gender and number agreement. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3 (2), 213–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillon-Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A. & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (8), 1870–1887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem?. Second Language Research, 28 (2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Pagliarini, E., & Stucchi, N. (2017). Language, Reading, and Motor Control: Get Rhythm!. Biolinguistics, 11 1, 395–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hao, J., & Chondrogianni, V. (2023). Comprehension and production of non-canonical word orders in Mandarin-speaking child heritage speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 13 (4), 468–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., de Carvalho, A., Fiévet, A. C., & Christophe, A. (2019). Three-to four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word meanings. Child Development, 90 (1), 82–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2015). Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53 (3), 277–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N. (2018). Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40 (1), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Brouwer, S. (2015). Delayed anticipatory spoken language processing in adults with dyslexia – evidence from eye-tracking. Dyslexia, 21 (2), 97–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Lachmann, T., Reis, A., & Petersson, K. M. (2018). Distinguishing cause from effect – many deficits associated with developmental dyslexia may be a consequence of reduced and suboptimal reading experience. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33 (3), 333–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31 (1), 19–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Pickering, M. J. (2019). Literacy advantages beyond reading: Prediction of spoken language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23 (6), 464–475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Igoa, J. M., García-Albea, J. E., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (1999). Gender-number dissociations in sentence production in Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 11 (1), 163–196.Google Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J. (2018). A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21 (2), 251–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., & Pickering, M. J. (2021). Automaticity and prediction in non-native language comprehension. In Kaan, E. & Grüter, T. (Eds.), Prediction in Second-language Processing and Learning (pp. 25–46). John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., & Grüter, T. (2021). Prediction in Second Language Processing and Learning. John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karaca, F., Brouwer, S., Unsworth, S. & Huettig, F. (2021). Prediction in bilingual children; The missing piece of the puzzle. In Kaan, E. & Grüter, T. (Eds.), Prediction in Second-Language Processing and Learning (pp. 115–137). John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., Halberda, J., Wood, J., & Carey, S. (2006). Acquisition of English number marking: The singular-plural distinction. Language Learning and Development, 2 (1), 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ladas, A. I., D. J. Carroll & A. B. Vivas. (2015). Attentional processes in low socioeconomic status bilingual children: Are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience?. Child Development, 86 (2), 557–578. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lê, S., Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 25 (1), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemmerth, N., & Hopp, H. (2019). Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic influences. Language Acquisition, 26 (1), 21–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18 (3), 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (4), 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mani, N., & Huettig, F. (2012). Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – But only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38 (4), 843–847.Google Scholar
(2014). Word reading skill predicts anticipation of upcoming spoken language input: A study of children developing proficiency in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126 1, 264–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marini, A., Marotta, L., Bulgheroni, S. & Fabbro, F. (2015). Batteria per la valutazione del linguaggio in bambini dai 4 ai 12 anni (BVL_4–12). Giunti O.S.Google Scholar
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69 (4), 574–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martinez-Nieto, L., & Restrepo, M. A. (2023). Grammatical gender in Spanish child heritage speakers: Incomplete or different acquisition?. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 13 (2), 267–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meir, N., Parshina, O., & Sekerina, I. A. (2020). The interaction of morphological cues in bilingual sentence processing: an eye-tracking study. In M. M. Brown & A. Kohut, BUCLD44: Proceedings of the 44th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 376–389). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2018). Early child second language acquisition: French gender in German children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21 (4), 656–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F. (2012). Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5 (1), 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitsugi, S., & Macwhinney, B. (2016). The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (1), 19–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, L., Paolieri, D., Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Gerfen, C., & Bajo, M. T. (2016). The gender congruency effect during bilingual spoken-word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (2), 294–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mornati, G., Riva, V., Vismara, E., Molteni, M., & Cantiani, C. (2022). Infants aged 12 months use the gender feature in determiners to anticipate upcoming words: an eye-tracking study. Journal of Child Language, 50 (4), 1–19.Google Scholar
Ng, S., Payne, B. R., Stine-Morrow, E. A., & Federmeier, K. D. (2018). How struggling adult readers use contextual information when comprehending speech: Evidence from event related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 125 1, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pagliarini, E., Scocchia, L., Granocchio, E., Sarti, D., Stucchi, N., & Guasti, M. T. (2020). Timing anticipation in adults and children with Developmental Dyslexia: evidence of an inefficient mechanism. Scientific Reports, 10 (1), 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panzini, A. (2017). Grammatica italiana. Edizioni Lindau.Google Scholar
Persici, V., Stucchi, N., & Arosio, F. (2019). Rhythmic and Morphosyntactic Predictions: The Anticipation Abilities of Italian Children with Developmental Dyslexia. In M. M. Brown & B. Dailey, BUCLD43: Proceedings of the 43rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 537–548). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144 (10), 1002–1044. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2020). E-Prime 3.0.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The EZ Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26 (4), 445–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Verbal and nonverbal predictors of language mediated anticipatory eye movements. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 77 (3), 720–730. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2007). DDE-2. Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva. Giunti OS.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, N. & Hulstijn, J. (2005). Automaticity in bilingualism and second language learning. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (pp. 371–388). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Soto-Corominas, A. (2021). Acquisition of quantified partitivity in Catalan-Spanish bilingualism: Influence from child-level and language-level factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11 (6), 873–908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Staub, A., & Clifton Jr, C. (2006). Syntactic prediction in language comprehension: evidence from either... or. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32 (2), 425–436.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The interpretability hypothesis: Evidence from wh- interrogatives in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 23 (2), 215–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A., & Tsimpli, I. (2014). The role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 (4), 765–805. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31 (3), 443–467.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska–Macgregor, M. & Leung, Y. K. I. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (1), 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

Baron, Alisa, Katrina Connell, Daniel Kleinman, Lisa M. Bedore & Zenzi M. Griffin
2024. Grammatical gender in spoken word recognition in school-age Spanish monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual children. Frontiers in Psychology 15 DOI logo
Meir, Natalia, Olga Parshina & Irina A. Sekerina
2024. Prediction in bilingual sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 544 ff. DOI logo
Pontikas, George, Ian Cunnings & Theodoros Marinis
2024. Sentence processing in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 476 ff. DOI logo
van Dijk, Chantal, Jasmijn Bosch & Sharon Unsworth
2024. Language processing in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 435 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.