References (40)
References
Aldrich, A. C. (2014). Acquisition of L2 phonology: An acoustic analysis of the centralization of L2 Spanish /a/ in adult L1 English-speaking learners. MA Thesis. Brigham Young University.
Amengual, M., & Simonet, M. (2020). Language dominance does not always predict cross-linguistic interactions in bilingual speech production. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 10 (6), 847–872. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bland, J. (2016). Speech style, syllable stress, and the second-language acquisition of Spanish /e/ and /o/. MA thesis, Virginia Polytechnic Institute; State University.
Botezatu, M. R., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F. (2024). Proficiency in a second language influences processing of print–to–sound mappings. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 14 (3), 285–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlucci, L., & Case, J. (2013). On the necessity of U-shaped learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5 (1), 56–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casani, E. (2020). Valutare la competenza morfosintattica in italiano L2. Una validazione corpus-based dei livelli del QCER. In E. Nuzzo, E. Santoro, & I. Vedder (Eds.), Valutazione e misurazione delle produzioni orali e scritte in italiano lingua seconda (pp. 15–26). Franco Cesati Editore.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. B. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development, 39 (2), 433–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cobb, K. (2009). La pronunciación de vocales átonas en español: La aplicación de reglas fonológicas por parte de hablantes no-nativos del español. MA Thesis. The University of Arizona.
Cobb, K., & Simonet, M. (2015). Adult second language learning of Spanish vowels. Hispania, 98 (1), 47–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coretta, S. (2023). Tidygam: Tidy prediction and plotting of Generalised Additive Models. \url{[URL]}
de la Fuente Iglesias, M., & Pérez Castillejo, S. (2022). L1 phonetic permeability and phonetic path towards a potential merger: The case of Galician mid vowels in bilingual production. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12 (2), 191–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin, S. M., & Miller, W. R. (1963). Language development. In H. W. Stevenson, J. C. Kagan, C. C. Spiker, N. B. Henry, & H. G. Richey (Eds.), Child psychology: The sixty-second yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part 1 (pp. 108–143). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second language acquisition. An introductory course. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeslin, K. L., & Guijarro-Fuentes, P. (2006). A longitudinal study of copula choice: Following development in variable structures. In N. Sagarra & A. J. Toribio (Eds.), Selected procedures of the 9th hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 144–156). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (2004). Mindless statistics. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 33 (5), 587–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (1986). Generalized Additive Models. Statistical Science, 1 (3), 297–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iruela, A. (1997). Adquisición del vocalismo español por holandeses: Análisis en estilo semiespontáneo. Estudios de Fonética Experimental IX 1, 135–180.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1985). If at first you do succeed... In S. M. Gass & C. G. Madden (Eds.), Input in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 345–353). Newbury.Google Scholar
Lightbown, P. (1983). Exploring relationships between developmental and instructional sequences in L2 acquisition. In H. Seliger & M. Long (Eds.), Classroom oriented research in second language acquisition (pp. 217–243). Newbury House.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1990). The least a second language acquisition theory needs to explain. TESOL Quarterly, 24 (4), 649–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D. (2018). Ggeffects: Tidy data frames of marginal effects from regression models. Journal of Open Source Software, 3 (26), 772. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., Xu, F., & Clahsen, H. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (4), i–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martínez Celdrán, E. (1984). Fonética. Teide.Google Scholar
McElreath, R. (2019). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Pearl, J. (2010). An introduction to causal inference. The International Journal of Biostatistics, 6 (2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, E. J., Miller, D. L., Simpson, G. L., & Ross, N. (2019). Hierarchical generalized additive models in ecology: An introduction with mgcv. PeerJ, 7 1, e6876. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., & Marinis, T. (2013). Processing empty categories in a second language: When naturalistic exposure fills the (intermediate) gap. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (1), 167–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [URL]
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shirai, Y. (1990). U-shaped behavior in L2 acquisition. Variability in Second Language Acquisition: Proceedings of the Tenth Meeting of the Second Language Research Forum, 2 1, 685–700.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. (2023). What do p-values mean in generalized additive model? Cross Validated.Google Scholar
Sóskuthy, M. (2017). Generalised additive mixed models for dynamic analysis in linguistics: A practical introduction. arXiv.org preprint, arXiv:1703.05339.Google Scholar
(2021). Evaluating generalised additive mixed modelling strategies for dynamic speech analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 84 1, 101017. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tamminga, M., Ahern, C., & Ecay, A. (2016). Generalized additive mixed models for intraspeaker variation. Linguistics Vanguard, 2 (s1), 33–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wieling, M. (2018). Analyzing dynamic phonetic data using generalized additive mixed modeling: A tutorial focusing on articulatory differences between L1 and L2 speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 70 1, 86–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, S., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., & Vulchanova, M. (2022). U-shaped trajectories in an L2 context: Evidence from the acquisition of verb morphology. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 19 1, 223–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. arXiv:1308.5499. [URL]
(2020). Statistics for linguists: An introduction using R. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wood, S. (2006). Generalized additive models: An introduction with R. CRC Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B), 73 (1), 3–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar