References (37)
References
Baker, R., Doran, R., McNabb, Y., Larson, M., & Ward, G. (2009). On the Non-Unified Nature of Scalar Implicature: An Empirical Investigation. International Review of Pragmatics, 1 (2), 211–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beltrama, A., & Xiang, M. (2013). Is good better than excellent? An experimental investigation on scalar implicatures and gradable adjectives. In E. Chemla, V. Homer & G. Winterstein (Eds), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 17 1, (pp. 81–98).Google Scholar
Benz, A., Bombi, C., & Gotzner, N. (2018). Scalar diversity and negative strengthening. In U. Sauerland, & S. Solt (Eds). Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 22, Vol. 11, (pp. 191–204). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, L. (1985). Dialogue games. Springer eBooks. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cho, J. (2024). Scalar implicatures in adult L2 learners: A self-paced reading study. Second Language Research, 02676583221134058. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cummins, C., & Rohde, H. (2015). Evoking Context with Contrastive Stress: Effects on Pragmatic Enrichment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 1. [URL]. DOI logo
Doran, R., Ward, G., McNabb, Y., Larson, M., & Baker, R. E. (2012). A novel paradigm for distinguishing between what is said and what is implicated. Language, 881, 124–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupuy, L., Stateva, P., Andreetta, S., Cheylus, A., Déprez, V., Henst, J.-B. van der, Jayez, J., Stepanov, A., & Reboul, A. (2019). Pragmatic abilities in bilinguals: The case of scalar implicatures. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9 (2), 314–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Planning and task-based performance: Theory and research. In Ellis, R. (Ed.), Planning and task performance in a second language (pp. 3–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feng, S., & Cho, J. (2019). Asymmetries between direct and indirect scalar implicatures in second language acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gotzner, N., Solt, S., & Benz, A. (2018). Scalar diversity, negative strengthening, and adjectival semantics. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English (Ph.D. dissertation). University of California Los Angeles.
Huang, Y. T., Spelke, E., & Snedeker, J. (2013). What exactly do numbers mean? Language Learning and Development, 9 (2), 105–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, C. (2007). Vagueness and grammar: the semantics of relative and absolute gradable adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30 (1), 1–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, C., & McNally, L. (2005). Scale structure, degree modification, and the semantics of gradable predicates. Language, 81 (2), 345–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Khorsheed, A., Rashid, S. M., Nimehchisalem, V., Imm, L. G., Price, J., & Ronderos, C. R. (2022). What second-language speakers can tell us about pragmatic processing. PLOS ONE, 17 (2): e0263724. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leech, G. (1983). Principles of pragmatics (1st ed.). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leffel, T., Cremers, A., Gotzner, N., & Romoli, J. (2019). Vagueness in implicature: The case of modified adjectives. Journal of Semantics, 36 (2), 317–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, Y. (2016). Processing of scalar inferences by Mandarin learners of English: An online measure. PloS One, 11 (1): e0145494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mazzaggio, G., Panizza, D., & Surian, L. (2021). On the interpretation of scalar implicatures in first and second language. Journal of Pragmatics, 171 1, 62–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meister, I. G., Buelte, D., Sparing, R., & Boroojerdi, B. (2007). A repetition suppression effect lasting several days within the semantic network. Experimental Brain Research, 183 (3), 371–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, D., Giancaspro, D., Iverson, M., Rothman, J., & Slabakova, R. (2016). Not just algunos, but indeed unos L2ers can acquire scalar implicatures in L2 Spanish. In A. Alba de la Fuente, E. Valenzuela, & C. Martínez Sanz (Eds.), Language Acquisition Beyond Parameters: Studies in honour of Juana M. Liceras (pp. 125–145). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pankratz, E., & Tiel, B. V. (2021). The role of relevance for scalar diversity: A usage-based approach. Language and Cognition, 13 (4), 562–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics- pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86 (3), 253–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rotstein, C., & Winter, Y. (2004). Total Adjectives vs. Partial Adjectives: Scale Structure and Higher-Order Modifiers. Natural Language Semantics, 12 (3), 259–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2010). Scalar implicatures in second language acquisition. Lingua, 120 1, 2444–2462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smeets, L., & Meroni, L. (2020). Stress or context for the computation of scalar implicatures. In P. Guijarro-Fuentes & C. Suárez-Gómez (Eds.), New Trends in Language Acquisition Within the Generative Perspective (pp. 313–331). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snape, N. & Hosoi, H. (2018). Acquisition of scalar implicatures: Evidence from adult Japanese L2 learners of English. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8 1, 163–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solt, S. (2011). Notes on the comparison class. In R. Nouwen, R. van Rooij, U. Sauerland, & H.-C. Schmitz (Eds.), Vagueness in Communication (pp. 189–206). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1 (1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Syrett, K., Austin, J., Sanchez, L., Germak, C., Lingwall, A., Perez-Cortes, S., Arias-Amaya, A., & Baker, H. (2016). The influence of conversational context and the developing lexicon on the calculation of scalar implicatures: Insights from Spanish–English bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7 (2), 230–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A. (2011). Proficiency assessment standards in second language acquisition research: “closing” the gap. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33 (3), 339–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Tiel, B., van Miltenburg, E., Zevakhina, N., & Geurts, B. (2016). Scalar diversity. Journal of Semantics, 33 (1), 137–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar