References
Dąbrowska, E
(2012) Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(3), 219–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, C.-H., Lidz, J., & Musolino, J
(2007) V-Raising and grammar competition in Korean: Evidence from negation and quantifier scope. Linguistic Inquiry, 38(1, 1–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hofmeister, P., Staum Casasanto, L., & Sag, I.A
(2012a) How do individual cognitive differences relate to acceptability judgments?: A reply to Sprouse, Wagers, and Phillips. Language, 88(2, 390–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hofmeister, P., Staum Casasanto, L., & Sag, I. A
(2012b) Misapplying working-memory tests: A reductio ad absurdum. Language, 88(2, 408–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W
(2005) Syntactic carpentry: An emergentist approach to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Kwak, H.-Y., Lee, O.-S., & Lee, M
(2011) An emergentist perspective on heritage language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 223–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Lee, M., & Kwak, H.-Y
(2009) Emergentism and second language acquisition. In W. Ritchie, & T. Bhatia (eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 69–88). Bingley, UK: Emerald Press.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Wagers, M., & Phillips, C
(2012a) A test of the relation between working-memory capacity and syntactic island effects. Language, 88(1, 82–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Working-memory capacity and island effects: A reminder of the issues and the facts. Language, 88(2, 401–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar