Bibliography

Bibliography

Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B.
(1989) Functionalism and the Competition Model. In B. MacWhinney and E. Bates (Eds.). The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S.
(1991) Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, 597–611. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.D.
(1978) Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 9, 427–73.Google Scholar
Hausser, R.
(1999) Foundations of computational linguistics: Man-machine communication in natural language. Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R., MacWhinney, B.
et al. (Eds.) (2008) Embodiment, ego-space, and action. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(1975) Pragmatic patterns in child syntax. Stanford Papers And Reports on Child Language Development 10, 153–165.Google Scholar
(1982) Basic syntactic processes. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language acquisition: Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics (pp. 73–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2008) How mental models encode embodied linguistic perspectives. In R. Klatzky, B. MacWhinney, & M. Behrmann (Eds.). Embodiment, ego-space, and action (pp. 369–410). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2012) The logic of the unified model. In S. Gass, & A. Mackey (Ed.). The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 211–227). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
in press). Item-based patterns in early syntactic development. In T. Herbst Ed. Valency relations Berlin Springer
McClelland, J.L., McNaughton, B.L.
, et al. (1995) Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review 102, 419–457. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W.
(2005) Syntactic carpentry. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar