References (15)
References
Carroll, S.E. (1989). Language acquisition studies and a feasible theory of grammar. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 341, 399–418. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1982). Spracherwerb in der Kindheit: Eine Untersuchung zur Entwicklung der Syntax bei Kleinkindern. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1986). The availability of universal grammar to adult and child learners: A study of the acquisition of German word order. Second Language Research, 21, 93–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flores, C. (2008). A competência sintáctica de falantes bilingues luso-alemães regressados a Portugal: Um estudo sobre erosão linguística. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidade do Minho, Braga.
. (2010). The effects of age on language attrition: Evidence from bilingual returnees. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 533–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamsson, N. (2003). Maturational constraints in second language acquisition. In C. Doughty, & M.H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 539–588). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroffke, S., Rothweiler, M., & Babur, E. (2007). Turkish-German successive bilingual children: Age of onset and the development of German V2. Paper presented at the 6th International Symposium on Bilingualism, Hamburg, May 2007.Google Scholar
Meisel, J.M. (1990). Infl-ection: Subjects and subject-verb agreement. In J.M. Meisel (Ed.), Two first languages: Early grammatical development in bilingual children (pp. 237–298). Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2011). First and second language acquisition: Parallels and differences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2013a). Sensitive phases in successive language acquisition: The critical period hypothesis revisited. In C. Boeckx, & K. Grohmann (Eds.), Handbook of biolinguistics (pp. 69–85). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2013b). Heritage language learners: Unprecedented opportunities for the study of grammars and their development? Theoretical Linguistics, 391, 225–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J.M., Elsig, M., & Bonnesen, M. (2011). Delayed acquisition of grammar in first language development: Subject-verb inversion and subject clitics in French interrogatives. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11, 347–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sopata, A. (2009). Erwerbstheoretische und glottodidaktische Aspekte des frühen Zweitspracherwerbs: Sprachentwicklung der Kinder im natürlichen und schulischen Kontext. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.Google Scholar
. (2011). Placement of infinitives in successive child language acquisition. In E. Rinke, & T. Kupisch (Eds.), The development of grammar: Language acquisition and diachronic change (105–121). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M. (this volume). Early, late or very late? Timing acquisition and bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism.
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Logue, Susan, Christina Sevdali, Raffaella Folli & Juliana Gerard
2024. The impact of internal and external factors across language domains and features in sequential bilingual acquisition. Journal of Child Language  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.