References
Arnon, I., & Ramscar, M
(2012) Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned. Cognition, 1221, 292–305. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G.S., & Bock, K
(2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 1131, 234–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conway, C.M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S.S., & Pisoni, D.B
(2010) Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 1141, 356–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Thorpe, K., & Marchman, V.A
(2010) Blue car, red car: Developing efficiency in online interpretation of adjective-noun phrases. Cognitive Psychology, 601, 190–217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A., & Marchman, V.A
(2008) Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I.A. Sekerina, E.M. Fernández, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: Online methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97–135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., Tenenbaum, J.B., & Gibson, E
(2013) Learning and long-term retention of large-scale artificial languages. PLoS One, 8, e52500. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graf Estes, K.M., Evans, J., Alibali, M.W., & Saffran, J.R
(2007) Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 181, 254–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T.R
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hay, J.F., Pelucchi, B., Graf Estes, K., & Saffran, J.R
(2011) Linking sounds to meanings: Infant statistical learning in a natural language. Cognitive Psychology, 631, 93–106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L
(2002) How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 731, 418–433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 451, 337–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L.V
(2010) Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 611, 343–365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kurumada, C., Meylan, S.C., & Frank, M.C
(2013) Zipfian frequency distributions facilitate word segmentation in context. Cognition, 1271, 439–453. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lany, J
(2014) Judging words by their covers and the company they keep: Probabilistic cues support word learning. Child Development, 851, 1727–1739. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 181, 193–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., Pelucchi, B., & Saffran, J.R
(2011) Isolated words enhance statistical language learning in infancy. Developmental Science, 141, 1323–1329. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G.F., Fernandes, K.J., & Johnson, S.P
(2007) Infant rule learning facilitated by speech. Psychological Science, 181, 387–391. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Misyak, J.B., Christiansen, M.H., & Tomblin, J.B
(2010) Sequential expectations: The role of prediction-based learning in language. Topics in Cognitive Science, 21, 138–153. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L
(2015) The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M.L
(2008) Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 351, 185–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A
(2013) Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 201, 1–10.Google Scholar