References

References

Allopenna, P.D., Magnuson, J.S., & Tanenhaus, M.K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M., & Kamide, Y.
(2004) Now you see it, now you don’t: Mediating the mapping between language and the visual world. In J.M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action. Eye Movements and the Visual World (pp. 347–386). New York: Psychology Press.[ p. 536 ]Google Scholar
Brandt-Kobele, O.-C., & Höhle, B.
(2014) The detection of subject-verb agreement violations by German-speaking children: An eye-tracking study. Lingua, 144, 7–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P.E., Valdés Kroff, J.R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R.E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 353–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M.W.
(2007) The influence of recent scene events on spoken comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 519–543. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G.R.
(2013a) Predicting meaning: What the brain tells us about the architecture of language comprehension. Invited talk presented at the 26th Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference. March 21-23, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.Google Scholar
(2013b) The proactive comprehender: What event-related potentials tell us about the dynamics of reading comprehension. In B. Miller, L.E. Cutting, & P.M. McCardle, (Eds.), Unraveling reading Comprehension. Behavioral, Neurological, and Genetic Components (pp. 176–192). Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L.
(2015) The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I.A.
(2012) Time course of processing of grammatical agreement information by Russian children. Paper presented at the 37th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Boston University.Google Scholar
Sekerina, I.A., & Campanelli, L.
(2013) Real-time processing of gender and number agreement by heritage Russian speakers. Poster presented at the International Conference on Multilingualism: Linguistic Challenges and Neurocognitive Mechanisms.McGill University, Montréal.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Spivey-Knowlton, M.J., Eberhard, K.M., & Sedivy, J.C.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar