References
Abdulkarim, L.
(2001) Complex wh-questions and universal grammars: New evidence from the acquisition of negative barriers (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Bishop, D., Nation, K., & Patterson, K.
(2014) When words fail us: Insights into language processing from developmental and acquired disorders. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20120403.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1986) Barriers. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1995) The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Three factors in language design. Linguistic inquiry, 36(1), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dayal, V.
(2000) Scope marking: Cross-linguistic variation in indirect dependency. In U. Lutz, G. Muller, & A. Von Stechow (Eds.), Wh-scope marking (Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 37) (pp.157–193). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G.
(2001) Continuity and modularity in language acquisition and research. In L. Santelmann, M. Verrips, & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Annual Review of Language Acquisition (Vol. 3, pp.1–64). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) On building up a sufficient representation for belief: Tense, Point of View and Wh-movement. In J. Costa, A. Castro, M. Lobo, & F. Pratas (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of Gala 2009 (pp.121–134). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.
(2015) Parsing and grammar: On filling in the gaps. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 459–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A.
(2009) Complements enable representation of the contents of false belief: evolution of a theory of Theory of Mind. In S. Foster-Cohen (Ed.), Language acquisition (pp.169–195). Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., Kotfila, J. & Roeper, T.
(submitted) When is recursion easier for children? In Proceedings of GALA 17. Dordrecht: Springer. Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E.
(2002) Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1037–1060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., Roeper, T., Harrington, E., & Gadilauskas, E.
(2012) Tense and truth in children’s question answering. In A. Biller, E. Chung, & A. Kimball (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.152–163). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & Roeper, T.
(1995a) Barriers, binding and acquisition of the DP/NP distinction. Language Acquisition, 4, 73–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995b) Relative clauses are barriers to wh-Movement for young children. Journal of Child Language, 22, 389–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., Roeper, T., Bland-Stewart, L., & Pearson, B. Z.
(2008) Answering hard questions: Wh-movement across dialects and disorder. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 67–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., Roeper, T., & Vainikka. A.
(1990) The acquisition of long-distance rules. In L. Frazier & J. G. de Villiers (Eds.), Language Processing and Language Acquisition (pp.257–297). Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H., & Tomasello. M.
(2001) The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A corpus-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 12, 97–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, G.
(2005) Partial wh-movement. In M. Everaert & H. van Riemsdijk (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax, Vol. 3. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Golinkoff., R., de Villiers, J. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Iglesias, A., & Wilson, M
(2017) QUILS: Quick Interactive Language Screener. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H., Foley, M., & Sedivy, J.
(1992) Adjunct islands and acquisition. In H. Goodluck & M. Rochemont (Eds.), Island constraints (pp.181–194). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrigan, K., Hacquard, V., & Lidz, J.
(2016) Syntactic bootstrapping in the acquisition of attitude verbs: Think, want and hope. In K. Kim, P. Umbal, T. Block, Q. Chan, T. Cheng, K. Finney, M. Katz, S. Nickel-Thompson, & L. Shorten (Eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL 33 (pp.196–206). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hyams, N.
(1986) Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, S., Lidz, J., & Hacquard, V.
(2012) The semantics and pragmatics of belief reports in preschoolers. In A. Chereches (Ed.), Proceedings of SALT 22 (pp. 247 – 267). Linguistic Society of America. < [URL]Google Scholar
McDaniel, D.
(1989) Partial and multiple wh-movement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 7(4), 565–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, D. Chiu, B., & Maxfield, T.
(1995) Parameters for wh-movement types: Evidence from child language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 709–753. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyagawa, S.
(2005) EPP and semantically vacuous scrambling. In J. Sabel & M. Saito (Eds.), The free word order phenomenon: Its syntactic sources and diversity (pp.181–220). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Oiry, M., & Demirdache, H.
(2006) Evidence from L1 acquisition for the syntax of wh-scope marking in French. In V. Torrens & L. Escobar (Eds.), The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages (Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 41) (pp.289–315). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Omaki, A.
(2010) Commitment and flexibility in the developing parser (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Omaki, A., Davidson White, I., Goro, T., Lidz, J., & Phillips, C.
(2014) No fear of commitment: Children’s incremental interpretation in English and Japanese wh-questions. Language Learning and Development, 10(3), 206–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philip, W., & de Villiers, J. G.
(1992) Monotonicity and the acquisition of weak islands. In E. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the twenty-fourth annual child language research forum (pp.99–111). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L.
(2015) The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 409–453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L.
(1991) Relativized minimality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1997) The fine structure of the left periphery. In L. Haegeman (Ed.), Elements of grammar (pp.281–337). Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T., & de Villiers, J. G.
(1994) Lexical links in the Wh-chain. In B. C. Lust, G. Hermon, & J. Kornfilt (Eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: Cross linguistic perspectives, Vol. II: Binding, dependencies and learnability (pp.357–390). Hillsdale, NJ; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2011) The acquisition path for wh-questions. In J. G. de Villiers, & T. Roeper (Eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp.189–246). Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, B.
(2004) A Minimalist account of partial wh-movement. Unpublished manuscript, University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T., & de Villiers, J. G.
(2005) DELV-NR (Diagnostic evaluation of language variation) norm-referenced test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Szabolsci, A., & Zwarts, F.
(1992) Unbounded dependencies and the algebraic semantics. Lecture notes of the Third European Summer School in Logic, Language and Information. Saarbrücken, Germany, August 1991.Google Scholar
Thornton, R.
(2008) Why continuity. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 26(1), 107–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K.
(1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106(1–4), 23–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

de Villiers, Jill, Jessica Kotfila & Tom Roeper
2020. When Is Recursion Easier for Children?. In New Trends in Language Acquisition Within the Generative Perspective [Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, 49],  pp. 239 ff. DOI logo
Liter, Adam, Elaine Grolla & Jeffrey Lidz
2022. Cognitive inhibition explains children’s production of medial wh-phrases. Language Acquisition 29:3  pp. 327 ff. DOI logo
Lutken, C. Jane, Géraldine Legendre & Akira Omaki
2020. Syntactic Creativity Errors in Children's Wh‐Questions. Cognitive Science 44:7 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.