References
Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H.
(1993) The CELEX Lexical Database (CD ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Bach, E.
(1962) The order of elements in a transformational grammar of German. Language, 38, 263–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M.
(2008) An alternative perspective on “semantic P600” effects in language comprehension. Brain Research Reviews, 59, 55–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen H., & Featherston S.
(1999) Antecedent priming at trace positions: Evidence from German scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 415–437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clifton, C. Jr., & Frazier, L.
(1989) Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In M. Tanenhaus & G. Carlson (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 273–317). Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dallas, A., DeDe, G., & Nicol, J.
(2013) An event‐related potential (ERP) investigation of filler‐gap processing in native and second language speakers. Language Learning, 63, 766–799. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Felser, C., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F.
(2003) Storage and integration in the processing of filler-gap dependencies: An ERP study of topicalization and wh-movement in German. Brain and Language, 87, 345–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D.
(2002) Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: The processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 250–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D.
(2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., & Frisch, S.
(2000) Verb argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 476–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garnsey, S. M., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chapman, R. M.
(1989) Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 51–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson E., & Hickok, G.
(1993) Sentence processing with empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 147–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E., & Warren, T.
(2004) Reading-time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies. Syntax, 7, 55–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J.
(1993) The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hestvik, A., Bradley, E., & Bradley, C.
(2012) Working memory effects of gap-predictions in normal adults: An event-related potentials study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 425–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hestvik, A., Maxfield, N., Schwartz, R. G., & Shafer, V.
(2007) Brain responses to filled gaps. Brain and Language, 100, 301–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jessen, A., & Felser, C.
(2019) Reanalysing object gaps during non-native sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs. Second Language Research, 35, 285–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jessen, A., Festman, J., Boxell, O., & Felser, C.
(2017) Native and non-native speakers’ brain responses to filled indirect object gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 1319–1338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P.
(2000) The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 159–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H.
(2002) The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 50–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, G.
(2001) Order preservation, parallel movement, and the emergence of the unmarked. In G. Legendre, J. Grimshaw, & S. Vikner (Eds.), Optimality-theoretic syntax (pp. 279–313). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H.
(2002) Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 531–571. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicol, J.
(1993) Reconsidering reactivation. In G. Altmann & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Cognitive models of speech processing: The second Sperlonga meeting (pp. 321–350). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J.
(1992) Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Barry, G.
(1991) Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 229–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. H.
(2005) ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 407–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rommers, J., & Federmeier, K. D.
(2017) Electrophysiological methods. In A. M. B. de Groot & P. Hagoort (Eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide (pp. 247–265). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J. E.
(2012) On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120, 135–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stowe, L. A.
(1986) Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanner, D., Norton, J. J., Morgan-Short, K., & Luck, S. J.
(2016) On high-pass filter artifacts (they’re real) and baseline correction (it’s a good idea) in ERP/ERMF analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 265, 166–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van de Koot, H., Silva, R., Felser, C., & Sato, M.
(2015) Does Dutch A-scrambling involve movement? Evidence from antecedent priming. The Linguistic Review, 32, 739–776. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Herten, M., Kolk, H., & Chwilla, D.
(2005) An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 241–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Hestvik, Arild, Baila Epstein, Richard G. Schwartz & Valerie L. Shafer
2022. Developmental Language Disorder as Syntactic Prediction Impairment. Frontiers in Communication 6 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.