Brain responses elicited by implausible fillers and filled object gaps in German
We report the results from two experiments on the processing of filler-gap dependencies in German using event-related potentials (ERPs). Our aim was to identify and isolate brain responses linked to semantic vs. syntactic integration processes. Using maximally parallel stimulus materials, we examined ERPs at the subcategorising verb in sentences containing either a semantically implausible direct object filler or a filled direct object gap. Whilst the processing difficulty associated with trying to integrate an implausible filler with its subcategoriser was reflected in an enhanced N400 response, encountering a filled preverbal object gap elicited a P600 response at the verb but no negativity. These results confirm that the semantic and syntactic integration of displaced constituents are dissociable and qualitatively distinct processes.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Theoretical background
- 3.The current study
- Materials
- Plausibility manipulation
- Filled-gap manipulation
- Participants
- Procedure
- EEG recording and analysis
- Results
- Behavioural results
- ERP results for implausible fillers
- ERP results for filled gaps
- 4.Discussion
-
Acknowledgements
-
Notes
-
References
References (32)
References
Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
Bach, E. (1962). The order of elements in a transformational grammar of German. Language, 38, 263–269.
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2008). An alternative perspective on “semantic P600” effects in language comprehension. Brain Research Reviews, 59, 55–73.
Clahsen H., & Featherston S. (1999) Antecedent priming at trace positions: Evidence from German scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 415–437.
Clifton, C. Jr., & Frazier, L. (1989). Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In M. Tanenhaus & G. Carlson (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 273–317). Dordrecht: Springer.
Dallas, A., DeDe, G., & Nicol, J. (2013). An event‐related potential (ERP) investigation of filler‐gap processing in native and second language speakers. Language Learning, 63, 766–799.
Felser, C., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F. (2003). Storage and integration in the processing of filler-gap dependencies: An ERP study of topicalization and wh-movement in German. Brain and Language, 87, 345–354.
Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: The processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 250–272.
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78–84.
Friederici, A. D., & Frisch, S. (2000). Verb argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 476–507.
Garnsey, S. M., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chapman, R. M. (1989). Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 51–60.
Gibson E., & Hickok, G. (1993). Sentence processing with empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 147–161.
Gibson, E., & Warren, T. (2004). Reading-time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies. Syntax, 7, 55–78.
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439–483.
Hestvik, A., Bradley, E., & Bradley, C. (2012). Working memory effects of gap-predictions in normal adults: An event-related potentials study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 425–438.
Hestvik, A., Maxfield, N., Schwartz, R. G., & Shafer, V. (2007). Brain responses to filled gaps. Brain and Language, 100, 301–316.
Jessen, A., & Felser, C. (2019). Reanalysing object gaps during non-native sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs. Second Language Research, 35, 285–300.
Jessen, A., Festman, J., Boxell, O., & Felser, C. (2017). Native and non-native speakers’ brain responses to filled indirect object gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 1319–1338.
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 159–201.
Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 50–68.
Müller, G. (2001). Order preservation, parallel movement, and the emergence of the unmarked. In G. Legendre, J. Grimshaw, & S. Vikner (Eds.), Optimality-theoretic syntax (pp. 279–313). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 531–571.
Nicol, J. (1993). Reconsidering reactivation. In G. Altmann & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Cognitive models of speech processing: The second Sperlonga meeting (pp. 321–350). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806.
Pickering, M., & Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 229–259.
Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. H. (2005). ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 407–428.
Rommers, J., & Federmeier, K. D. (2017). Electrophysiological methods. In A. M. B. de Groot & P. Hagoort (Eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide (pp. 247–265). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell.
Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J. E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120, 135–162.
Stowe, L. A. (1986). Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227–245.
Tanner, D., Norton, J. J., Morgan-Short, K., & Luck, S. J. (2016). On high-pass filter artifacts (they’re real) and baseline correction (it’s a good idea) in ERP/ERMF analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 265, 166–170.
Van de Koot, H., Silva, R., Felser, C., & Sato, M. (2015). Does Dutch A-scrambling involve movement? Evidence from antecedent priming. The Linguistic Review, 32, 739–776.
Van Herten, M., Kolk, H., & Chwilla, D. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 241–255.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Hestvik, Arild, Baila Epstein, Richard G. Schwartz & Valerie L. Shafer
2022.
Developmental Language Disorder as Syntactic Prediction Impairment.
Frontiers in Communication 6
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.