Chapter published in:
L1 Acquisition and L2 Learning: The view from Romance
Edited by Larisa Avram, Anca Sevcenco and Veronica Tomescu
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 65] 2021
► pp. 356378
References

References

Ahern, A.
(2006) Spanish mood, metarepresentation and propositional attitudes. In K. von Heusinger & K. Turner (Eds.), Where semantics meets pragmatics (pp. 445–471). Brill.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M.
(1999) A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, A., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
(1988) Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6(4), 315–324. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bascelli, E., & Barbieri, M. S.
(2002) Italian children’s understanding of the epistemic and deontic modal verbs dovere (must) and potere (may). Journal of Child Language, 29(1), 87–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J.
(2006) The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Developmental Science, 9, 207–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R.
(1983) Mood selection among Spanish speaking children, ages 4 to 12. The Bilingual Review, 10, 21–32.Google Scholar
(1985) From research to the classroom. Hispania, 68, 66–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, D.
(1987) Semantic constraints on relevance. Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2002) Relevance and linguistic meaning: The semantics and pragmatics of discourse markers. Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosque, I.
(2012) Mood. Indicative vs. subjunctive. In J. I. Hualde, A. Olarrea, & E. O’Rourke (Eds.), Handbook of Hispanic linguistics (pp. 373–394). Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bybee-Hooper, J., & Terrell, T.
(1974) A semantically based analysis of mood in Spanish. Hispania, 57, 484–494. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(2007) Approaching UG from below. In U. Sauerland & H. Martin Gärtner (Eds.), Interfaces + Recursion = Language? (pp. 1–29). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Collentine, J.
(2003) The development of subjunctive and complex syntactic ability among foreign language learners of Spanish. In B. Lafford & R. Salaberry (Eds.), Spanish second language acquisition: The state of the science (pp. 74–97). Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Curcó, C.
(2000) Irony: Negation, echo and metarepresentation. Lingua, 110, 257–280. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J. G.
(2004) Getting complements on your mental state (Verbs). In J. van Kampen & S. Baauw (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 3 (pp. 13–26). LOT.Google Scholar
(2005) Can language acquisition give children a point of view? In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 186–219). Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) The interface of language and Theory of Mind. Lingua, 117(11), 1858–1878. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dracos, M., Requena, P., & Miller, K.
(2019) Acquisition of mood selection in Spanish-speaking children. Language Acquisition, 26(1), 106–118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Escandell-Vidal, V., Leonetti, M., & Ahern, A.
(2012) Procedural meaning: Problems and perspectives. Brill.Google Scholar
Fábregas, A.
(2014) A guide to subjunctive and modals in Spanish: Questions and analyses. Borealis: An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 3(2), 1–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Farkas, D.
(1992) On the semantics of subjunctive complements. In P. Hirschbühler & E. F. K. Koerner (Eds.), Romance languages and modern linguistic theory: Selected papers from the XX linguistic symposium on Romance Languages, University of Ottawa, April 10–14, 1990 (pp. 69–105). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Giannakidou, A.
(2013) (Non)veridicality, evaluation, and event actualization: evidence from the subjunctive in relative clauses. In M. Taboada & R. Trnavac (Eds.), Nonveridicality, perspective, and discourse coherence (pp. 17–50). Brill.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L.
(1990) The structural sources of verb meaning. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heizmann, T.
(2006) Acquisition of deontic and epistemic readings of must and müssen . In T. Heizmann (Ed.), UMOP 34: Current issues in language acquisition (pp. 21–50). GSLA.Google Scholar
Hernández-Pina, F.
(1984) Teorías psicosociolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Jesús, A., Marques, R., & Santos, A. L.
(2019) Semantic features in the acquisition of mood in European Portuguese. Language Acquisition, 26(3), 302–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kempchinsky, P.
(2009) What can the subjunctive disjoint reference effect tell us about the subjunctive? Lingua, 119, 1788–1810. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P.
(2002) Disjoint reference and the typology of pronouns. In I. Kaufmann & B. Stiebels (Eds.), More than words (pp. 179–226). Akademie Verlag. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S.
(1994) La adquisición de la lengua española. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Matsui, T., & Miura, Y.
(2009) Children’s understanding of certainty and evidentiality: Advantage of grammaticalized forms over lexical alternatives. In S. A. Fitneva & T. Matsui (Eds.), Evidentiality: A window into language and cognitive development (pp. 63–77). Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Moeschler, J.
(2016) Where is procedural meaning located? Evidence from discourse connectives and tenses. Lingua, 175–176, 122–138. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A.
(1998) The acquisition of modality: Implications for theories on semantic representation. Mind & Language, 13(3), 370–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Linking early linguistic and conceptual capacities: The role of theory of mind. In A. Cienki, B. Luka, & M. Smith (Eds.), Conceptual and discourse factors in linguistic structure (pp. 169–184). CSLI.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T.
(2001) Subjunctive mood in Spanish child relatives: At the interface of linguistic and cognitive development. In K. Nelson, A. Aksu-Koç, & C. Johnson (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 11, pp. 69–93). Laurence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2008) Subjuntivo y conciencia de la subjetividad en la adquisición infantil del lenguaje y la teoría de la mente. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 28, 90–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Zauner, P., & Sprung, M.
(2005) What does “that” have to do with point of view? Conflicting desires and “want” in German. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 220–244). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Quer, J.
(2001) Interpreting mood. Probus, 13, 81–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sánchez-Naranjo, J., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T.
(2010) In the wrong mood at the right time: Children’s acquisition of the Spanish subjunctive in temporal clauses. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 55(2), 227–255.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., & Wilcox, S. A.
(1991) Constraints on the acquisition of English modals. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 319–353). Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D.
(1995) Relevance: Communication and cognition. Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wilson, D.
(2000) Metarepresentation in linguistic communication. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 230–258). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D.
(1992) On verbal irony. Lingua, 87, 53–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) On verbal irony. In R. Gibbs & R. Colston (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 35–55). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar