References
Acquaviva, P.
2009The structure of the Italian declension system. In Selected Proceedings of the 6th Décembrettes, F. Montermini, G. Boyé & J. Tseng (eds), 50–62. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Antelmi, D.
1997La prima grammatica italiana. Bologna: il Mulino.Google Scholar
Arencibia, G. L.
2008Sprachdominanz bei bilingualen Kindern mit Deutsch und Französisch, Italienisch oder Spanisch als Erstsprachen. PhD dissertation, Bergische Universität Wuppertal.
Aronoff, M.
1994Morphology by Itself: Stems and Inflectional Classes. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & Rankin, J.
1979Morphological development in Italian: Connotation and denotation. Journal of Child Language 6(1): 29–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernandez, A. & Pizzamiglio, L.
1996Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics 58(7): 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bazalgette, T. O.
2015Algorithmic Acquisition of Focus Parameters. PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge.
Benati, A.
2004The effects of processing instruction and its components on the acquisition of gender agreement in Italian. Language Awareness 13(2): 67–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bianchi, G.
2013Gender in Italian-German bilinguals: A comparison with German L2 learners of Italian. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16(3): 538–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A., Roberts, I. & Sheehan, M.
2014Complexity in comparative syntax: The view from modern parametric theory. In Measuring Grammatical Complexity, F. J. Newmeyer & L. B. Preston (eds), 103–127. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boeckx, C.
2014What principles and parameters got wrong. In Linguistic Variation in the Minimalist Framework, M. C. Picallo (ed.), 155–178. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boloh, Y. & Ibernon, L.
2010Gender attribution and gender agreement in 4‑ to 10-year-old French children. Cognitive Development 25(1): 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bottari, P., Cipriani, P. & Chilosi, A.-M.
1993/1994Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition 3(4): 327–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cantone, K. F.
2007Code-switching in Bilingual Children. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cantone, K. F., Kupisch, T., Müller, N. & Schmitz, K.
2008Rethinking language dominance in bilingual children. Linguistische Berichte 215: 307–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caprin, C. & Guasti, M. T.
2009The acquisition of morphosyntax in Italian: A cross-sectional study. Applied Psycholinguistics 30(1): 23–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S. E.
1989Second language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning 39(4): 535–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2017Exposure and input in bilingual development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20(1): 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Bello, A., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S. & Pasqualetti, P.
2015Il primo vocabolario del bambino: Gesti, parole e frasi. Valori di riferimento fra 8 e 36 mesi delle Forme complete e delle Forme brevi del questionario MacArthur-Bates CDI. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Leonard, L. B., Volterra, V. & Campagnoli, M. G.
1993Toward mastery of Italian morphology: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Child Language 20(2): 377–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chini, M.
1995aGenere grammaticale e acquisizione. Aspetti della morfologia nominale in italiano L2. Milano: Francoangeli.Google Scholar
1995bGrammatiche a confronto: La categoria grammaticale del genere nella competenza di nativi italofoni e nelle interlingue di apprendenti dell’italiano come L2. In L’universo delle lingue. Confrontare lingue e grammatiche nella scuola, Quaderni del Giscel, P. Desideri (ed.), 277–294. Firenze: La Nuova Italia.Google Scholar
1998Genuserwerb des Italienischen durch deutsche Lerner. In Eine zweite Sprache lernen, H. Wegener (ed.), 39–60. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
1995The Minimalist Program. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
2005Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36(1): 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2007Approaching UG from below. In Interfaces + Recursion = Language?: Chomsky’s Minimalism and the View from Syntax-Semantics, U. Sauerland & H.-M. Gärtner (eds), 1–29. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cipriani, P., Chilosi, A. M., Bottari, P. & Pfanner, L.
1993L’acquisizione della morfosintassi in ita-liano: Fasi e processi. Padua: Unipress.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G.
1991Gender. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006Gender, grammatical. In Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, K. Brown (ed.), 749–756. Oxford: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Martino, M., Bracco, G. & Laudanna, A.
2011The activation of grammatical gender information in processing Italian nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes 26(4–6): 745–776. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U. & Thornton, A. M.
1996Italian nominal inflection. Wiener Linguistische Gazette, 55–57, 1–24.Google Scholar
Eichler, N.
2011Code-Switching bei bilingualen Kindern. Eine Analyse der gemischtsprachlichen Nominalphrasen unter besonderer Berücksichtigung des Genus. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Eichler, N., Hager, M. & Müller, N.
2012Code-switching within the DP and gender assignment in bilingual children: French, Italian, Spanish and German. Zeitschrift für französische Sprache und Literatur 122(3): 227–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eichler, N., Jansen, V. & Müller, N.
2013Gender in French-German, Italian-German, Spanish-German and Italian-French children. International Journal of Bilingualism 17(5): 550–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ezeizabarrena, M-J. & Garcia, F. I.
2018Length of utterance, in morphemes or in Words? MLU3-w, a reliable measure of language development in early Basque. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 2265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, F.
2005A Syntactic Analysis of the Nominal Systems of Italian and Luganda: How Nouns Can Be Formed in the Syntax. PhD dissertation, New York University.
Fey, S.
2020Der Genuserwerb am Beispiel eines trilingualen Kindes: Spanisch, Italienisch und Französisch im Vergleich. Bachelor’s thesis, University of Hamburg.
Gudmundson, A.
2010L’acquisizione del genere grammaticale in italiano L2. Stockholm: US-AB.Google Scholar
Hager, M.
2014Der Genuserwerb bei mehrsprachig aufwachsenden Kindern – Eine longitudinale Untersuchung bilingualer und trilingualer Kinder der Sprachenkombinationen deutsch-französisch/italienisch/spanisch, französisch-italienisch/spanisch und deutsch-spanisch-katalanisch. PhD dissertation, Bergische Universität Wuppertal.
Hauser-Grüdl, N., Arencibia Guerra, L., Witzmann, F., Leray, E. & Müller, N.
2010Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual children: Can input frequency account for it? Lingua 120(11): 2638–2650. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holmberg, A. & Roberts, I.
2014Parameters and the three factors of language design. In Linguistic Variation in the Minimalist Framework, C. M. Picallo (ed.), 61–81. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivanova-Sullivan, T. & Sekerina, I. A.
2019Distributional regularity of cues facilitates gender acquisition: A contrastive study of two closely related languages. In Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, M. M. Brown & B. Dailey (eds), 311–323. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. & Faussart, C.
1998Gender agreement in the processing of spoken French. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27(6): 597–617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R.
2009Definite Markers, Phi Features and Agreement: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of the Amharic DP. PhD dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
Kupisch, T.
2006The Acquisition of Determiners in Bilingual German-Italian and German-French Children. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., Müller, N. & Cantone, K. F.
2002Gender in monolingual and bilingual first language acquisition: Comparing Italian and French. Lingue e Linguaggio 1(1): 107–149.Google Scholar
Lampitelli, N.
2010Nounness, gender, class and syntactic structures in Italian nouns. In Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory: Selected Papers from ‘Going Romance’ Groningen 2008, B. Bok-Bennema, B. Kampers-Mahne & B. Hollebrandse (eds), 195–214. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Caselli, M. C. & Devescovi, A.
2005Italian children’s use of verb and noun morphology during the preschool years. First Language 22(3): 287–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longobardi, G.
2006A minimalist program for parametric linguistics? In Organizing Grammar: Linguistic Studies in Honor of Henk van Riemsdijk, H. Broekhuis, N. Corver, R. Huybregts, U. Kleinhenz & J. Koster (eds), 407–414. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Loporcaro, M.
2018Gender from Latin to Romance. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Lowenstamm, J.
2007On little n, ROOT, and types of nouns. In The Sounds of Silence: Empty Elements in Syntax and Phonology, J. Hartmann, V. Hegedus & H. V. Riemsdijk (eds), 105–143. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
2012Feminine and gender, or why the feminine profile of French nouns has nothing to do with gender. In Linguistic Inspirations. Edmund Gussmann In Memoriam, E. Cyran, H. Kardela & B. Szymanek (eds), 371–406. Lublin: Wydawnictwo Katolicki Uniwersytet Lubelski.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
1978The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 43(1/2): 1–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. & Chalkley, M. A.
1980The internal language of children’s syntax: The nature and ontogenesis of syntactic categories. In Children’s Language, Vol. II, K. E. Nelson (ed.), 127–214. New York NY: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M.
2007The weaker language in early child bilingualism: Acquiring a first language as a second language? Applied Psycholinguistics 28(3): 495–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, A. E.
1986The Acquisition of Gender: A Study of English and German. Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., Arnaus Gil, L., Eichler, N., Geveler, J., Hager, M., Jansen, V., Patuto, M., Repetto, V. & Schmeißer, A.
2015Code-Switching: Spanisch, Italienisch, Französisch. Eine Einführung. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Müller, N., Kupisch, T., Schmitz, K. & Cantone, K. F.
2011Einführung in die Mehrsprachigkeitsforschung: Deutsch, Französisch, Italienisch. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. J.
2004Against a parameter-setting approach to typological variation. In Linguistic Variation Yearbook, P. Pica (ed.), 181–234. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
2017Where, if anywhere, are parameters? A critical historical overview of parametric theory. In On Looking into Words (and Beyond), C. Bowern, L. Horn & R. Zanuttini (eds), 547–569. Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Padovani, R. & Cacciari, C.
2003Il ruolo della trasparenza morfologica delle parole in Italiano. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 4: 749–771.Google Scholar
Pérez-Tattam, R., Ezeizabarrena, M. J., Stadthagen-González, H. & Mueller Gathercole, V. C.
2019Gender assignment to Spanish pseudowords by monolingual and Basque-Spanish bilingual children. Languages, 4(3), 58, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Picallo, C. M.
2014Linguistic Variation in the Minimalist Framework. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pillunat, A.
2007Der Erwerb des Lexikons durch mehrsprachige Kinder: Französisch, Italienisch und Deutsch im Vergleich. Master’s thesis, Bergische Universität Wuppertal.
Pizzuto, E. & Caselli, M. C.
1992The acquisition of Italian morphology: Implications for models of language development. Journal of Child Language 19(3): 491–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reeder, P. A., Newport, E. L. & Aslin, R. N.
2017Distributional learning of subcategories in an artificial grammar: Category generalization and subcategory restrictions. Journal of Memory and Language 97: 17–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, S.
2013Der Erwerb des Adjektivs bei bilingual deutsch-italienischen Kindern. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Roberts, I.
2019Parameter Hierarchies & Universal Grammar. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmeißer, A., Hager, M., Arnaus Gil, L., Jansen, V., Geveler, J., Eichler, N., Patuto, M. & Müller, N.
2016Related but different: The two concepts of language dominance and language proficiency. In Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Operationalization and Measurement, C. Silva-Corvalán & J. Treffers-Daller (eds), 36–65. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M., Minkov, M., Dieser, E., Protassova, E., Moin, V. & Polinsky, M.
2014Acquisition of Russian gender agreement by monolingual and bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism 19(6): 726–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serianni, L.
1988Grammatica italiana. Suoni forme costrutti. Torino: UTET.Google Scholar
Smith, N. & Law, A.
2009On parametric (and non-parametric) variation. Biolinguistics 3(4): 332 – 343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stahnke, Johanna
2022The Acquisition of French Determiners by Bilingual Children: A Prosodic Account. Languages 7: 200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teschner, R. & Russell, W.
1984The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An inversed dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics 1(1): 115–132.Google Scholar
Thornton, A. M.
2003L’assegnazione del genere in italiano. In Actas del XXIII Congreso Internacional de Lingüística y Filología Románica, Vol. I, F. Sánchez Miret (ed.), 467–481. Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009Constraining gender assignment rules. Language Sciences 31(1): 14–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thornton, A. M., Iacobini, C. & Burani, C.
1997BDVDB. Una base di dati sul Vocabolario di Base della lingua italiana. Roma: Bulzoni.Google Scholar
Tuite, K.
1997Svan. Languages of the World / Materials. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Yip, V. & Matthews, S.
2006Assessing language dominance in bilingual acquisition: A case for mean length utterance differentials. Language Assessment Quarterly 3(2): 97–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar