References

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D.
(2007) Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Acheson, D. J., & Hagoort, P.
(2014) Twisting tongues to test for conflict-monitoring in speech production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Allen, K., Pereira, F., Botvinick, M., & Goldberg, A. E.
(2012) Distinguishing grammatical constructions with fMRI pattern analysis. Brain and Language, 123(3), 174–182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y.
(1999) Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
American Academy of Arts and Sciences
(2017) America’s languages: Investing in language education for the 21st century. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts and Sciences.Google Scholar
Au, T. K., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S.-A., & Oh, J. S.
(2002) Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 13(3), 238–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bartolotti, J., Marian, V., Schroeder, S., & Shook, A.
(2011) Bilingualism and inhibitory control influence statistical learning of novel word forms. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 324. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D.
(2014) Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857–894. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baus, C., Costa, A., & Carreiras, M.
(2013) On the effects of second language immersion on first language production. Acta Psychologica, 142(3), 402–409. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., & Dussias, P. E.
(2020a) Codes-witching: A bilingual toolkit for opportunistic speech planning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1699. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo-Tamargo, R., & Kroll, J. F.
(2020b) Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 1022–1047. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Kroll, J. F.
(2017) The neurobiology of language: Looking beyond monolinguals. A special issue of Biolinguistics celebrating the 50-year anniversary of Eric Lenneberg’s Biological foundations of language. Biolinguistics, 11, 339–352.Google Scholar
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F.
(2015) Native language change during early stages of second language learning. NeuroReport, 26(16), 966–971. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2019) English only? Monolinguals in linguistically diverse contexts have an edge in language learning. Brain and Language, 196, 104644. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D.
(2018) Plasticity, variability and age in second language acquisition and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blank, I., Kanwisher, N., & Fedorenko, E.
(2014) A functional dissociation between language and multiple-demand systems revealed in patterns of BOLD signal fluctuations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112(5), 1105–1118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bogulski, C. A., Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F.
(2019) Bilingualism as a desirable difficulty: Advantages in word learning depend on regulation of the dominant language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 1052–1067. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boudewyn, M. A., Long, D. L., & Swaab, T. Y.
(2012) Cognitive control influences the use of meaning relations during spoken sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2659–2668. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B.
(2013) A novelty effect in phonetic drift of the native language. Journal of Phonetics, 41(6), 520–533. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1983) Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(4), 561–584.Google Scholar
Declerck, M.
(2020) What about proactive language control? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(1), 24–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M., & Philipp, A. M.
(2015) A review of control processes and their locus in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1630–1645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M.
(2000) The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499–533. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., van Jaarsveld, H., & Brinke, S. T.
(1998) Interlingual homograph recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(1), 51–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N.
(2007) The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 101–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. M., & Gally, J. A.
(2001) Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(24), 13763. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, P. E., Nigg, J. T., & Ferreira, F.
(2017) Executive function and intelligence in the resolution of temporary syntactic ambiguity: An individual differences investigation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(7), 1263–1281. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J.
(2004) Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 44 (p. 145–199). Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2017) Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12427. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E.
(2007) Language contact in bilingualism: Phonetic system interactions. In Hualde, J. I. & Cole, J. (Eds.), Laboratory phonology (Vol. 9, pp. 353–382). Berlin, New-York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S.
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(1), 78–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fricke, M., Zirnstein, M., Navarro-Torres, C., & Kroll, J. F.
(2019) Bilingualism reveals fundamental variation in language processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(1), 200–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E.
(2019) Explain me this: Creativity, competition, and the partial productivity of constructions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., Crinion, J., & Price, C. J.
(2006) Convergence, degeneracy, and control. Language Learning, 56(s1), 99–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Kroll, J. F.
(2019) The neurolinguistics of bilingualism. In G. de Zubicaray & N. Schiller (Eds.), Oxford handbook of neurolinguistics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Wei, L.
(2016) Code-switching and language control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 883–884. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1989) Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Bilingualism and Neurolinguistics, 36(1), 3–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., Baum, S., Klein, D., & Titone, D.
(2018) Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia, 117, 123–134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F.
(2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2300–2309. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, M., Jurafsky, D., & Futrell, R.
(2020) Universals of word order reflect optimization of grammars for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(5), 2347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T.
(2018) Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 892–916. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Cognate effects in picture naming: does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 106(1), 501–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N. S., Jaeggi, S. M., & Novick, J. M.
(2017) A common neural hub resolves syntactic and non-syntactic conflict through cooperation with task-specific networks. Brain and Language, 166, 63–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N. S., Kuchinsky, S. E., & Novick, J. M.
(2021) Direct impact of cognitive control on sentence processing and comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36(2), 211–239. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N. S., & Novick, J. M.
(2016) Dynamic engagement of cognitive control modulates recovery from misinterpretation during real-time language processing. Psychological Science, 27(4), 572–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. F., & Gennari, S. P.
(2014) Competitive mechanisms in sentence processing: Common and distinct production and reading comprehension networks linked to the prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage, 84, 354–366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesch, E., Tohkura, Y., Kettermann, A., & Siebert, C.
(2003) A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition, 87(1), B47–B57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, A., Fricke, M., & Kroll, J. F.
(2016) Cross-language activation begins during speech planning and extends into second language speech. Language Learning, 66(2), 324–353. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L.
(1989) Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V.
(2009) The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 705–710. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L.
(2015) Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 377–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., & Dufour, R.
(2002) The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 18(2), 137–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Takahesu Tabori, A., & Mech, E.
(2017) Beyond typical learning: Variation in language experience as a lens to the developing mind. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(6), 1336–1340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Tokowicz, N.
(2005) Models of bilingual representation and processing. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 531–553). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K.
(1993) Innate predispositions and the effects of experience in speech perception: The native language magnet theory. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. McNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 259–274). Dordrecht: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831–843. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T.
(2008) Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 979–1000. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P.
(2006) Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13–F21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P., Williams, K., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K., & Lindblom, B.
(1992) Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255(5044), 606. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H.
(1967) The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice, 2(12), 59–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A.
(2014) Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301–324. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G.
(2009) Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1507–1515. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2005) A unified model of language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 49–67). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(1994) The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101(4), 676–703. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Sprivey, M.
(2003) Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mason, P. H., Domínguez, D. J. F., Winter, B., & Grignolio, A.
(2015) Hidden in plain view: Degeneracy in complex systems. Biosystems, 128, 1–8. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A.
(2004) Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A.
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J.
(2011) Effects of cognate status on word comprehension in second language learners: An ERP investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1634–1647. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F.
(2012) When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 224–237. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morford, J. P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piñar, P., & Kroll, J. F.
(2011) When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition, 118(2), 286–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Navarro-Torres, C. A., Garcia, D. L., Chidambaram, V., & Kroll, J. F.
(2019) Cognitive control facilitates attentional disengagement during second language comprehension. Brain Sciences, 9(5), 95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nozari, N.
(2018) How special is language production? Perspectives from monitoring and control. In K. D. Federmeier & D. G. Watson (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 68, pp. 179–213). Cambridge: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J.
(2005) The neural basis of first and second language processing. Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 202–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., Dubins, M. H., Jasinska, K., & Shalinsky, M.
(2012) The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130–143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M., Di Biase, B., Kawaguchi, S., & Håkansson, G.
(2005) Processing constraints on L1 transfer. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 128–153). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Chen, J.-K., Delcenserie, A., Genesee, F., & Klein, D.
(2015) Past experience shapes ongoing neural patterns for language. Nature Communications, 6(1), 10073. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Klein, D., Chen, J. K., Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F.
(2014) Mapping the unconscious maintenance of a lost first language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(48), 17314-17319. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J.
(2015) Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological Science, 26(4), 490–498. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, E., Newman, S., Kroll, J. F., & Diaz, M. T.
(2018) Neural signatures of inhibitory control in bilingual spoken production. Cortex, 108, 50–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sabourin, L., Stowe, L. A., & de Haan, G. J.
(2006) Transfer effects in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research, 22(1), 1–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S.
(2010) Languages at play: The relevance of L1 attrition to the study of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(1), 1–7. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, A. I., Kroll, J. F., & Diaz, M.
(2007) Reading words in Spanish and English: Mapping orthography to phonology in two languages. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(1), 106–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scontras, G., Fuchs, Z., & Polinsky, M.
(2015) Heritage language and linguistic theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1545. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F.
(2012) A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23(9), 994–999. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stasenko, A., Hays, C., Wierenga, C. E., & Gollan, T. H.
(2020) Cognitive control regions are recruited in bilinguals’ silent reading of mixed-language paragraphs. Brain and Language, 204, 104754. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Werker, J. F.
(2007) Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C.
(1984) Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., Warrier, C. M., Penhune, V. B., Roy, A. K., Sadehh, A., Parrish, T. B., & Zatorre, R. J.
(2008) Volume of left Heschl’s gyrus and linguistic pitch learning. Cerebral Cortex, 18(4), 828–836. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2013) Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1781–1792. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O.
(2013) Network hubs in the human brain. Special Issue: The Connectome, 17(12), 683–696. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T.
(2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 780–789. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vuong, L. C., & Martin, R. C.
(2014) Domain-specific executive control and the revision of misinterpretations in sentence comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(3), 312–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zirnstein, M., Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F.
(2019) Variation in language experience shapes the consequences of bilingualism. In I. A. Sekerina, L. Spradlin, & V. V. Valian (Eds.), Bilingualism and executive function: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 35–48). Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins Publishers. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zirnstein, M., van Hell, J. G., & Kroll, J. F.
(2018) Cognitive control ability mediates prediction costs in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognition, 176, 87–106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar