Article published In:
Language, Interaction and Acquisition
Vol. 12:2 (2021) ► pp.210250
References (83)
References
Abdelli-Beruh, N. B. (2012). Voicing and devoicing assimilation of French /s/ and /z/. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41(5), 371–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baroni, A. (2014). Element theory and the magic of /s/. In E. Cyran & J. Szpyra Kozłowska (Eds.), Crossing phonetics-phonology lines (pp. 3–30). Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., Hayes-Harb, R., & Escudero, P. (2015). Second language phonology at the interface between acoustic and orthographic input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(1), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., Sokolovic-Perovic, M., Mairano, P., & Cerni, T. (2018). Orthography-induced length contrasts in the second language phonological systems of experienced speakers of English as a Second Language: Evidence from minimal pairs. Language and Speech, 61(4), 577–598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M., & Loporcaro, M. (2005). The sound pattern of Standard Italian, as compared with the varieties spoken in Florence, Milan and Rome. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 35(2), 132–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, C. (1995). A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 171–206). York: York Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In O. S. Bohn & M. Munro (Eds.), Second-language speech learning: The role of language experience in speech perception and production. A Festschrift in honour of James E. Flege (pp. 13–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 51, 341–345.Google Scholar
Bradley, T. G., & Delforge, A. M. (2006). Systemic contrast and the diachrony of Spanish sibilant voicing. In R. Gess & D. Arteaga (Eds.), Historical Romance linguistics: Retrospective and perspectives (p. 19–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broniś, O. (2016). Italian vowel paragoge in loanword adaptation. Phonological analysis of the Roman variety of Standard Italian. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 28(2), 25–68.Google Scholar
Brown, A. (1988). Functional load and the teaching of pronunciation. TESOL Quarterly, 22(4), 593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canepari, L., & Giovannelli, B. (2008). La buona pronuncia italiana del terzo millennio. Rome: Aracne.Google Scholar
Catford, J. C. (1987). Phonetics and the teaching of pronunciation: A systemic description of the teaching of English phonology. In J. Morley (Ed.), Current perspectives on pronunciation: Practices anchored in theory (pp. 83–100). Washington D.C.: TESOL.Google Scholar
Cole, R. A., & Cooper, W. E. (1975). Perception of voicing in English affricates and fricatives. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 58(6), 1280–1287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Contreras Roa, L., Mairano, P., Capliez, M. & Bouzon, C. (2020). Voice assimilation of morphemic -s in the L2 English of L1 French, L1 Italian and L1 Spanish learners. Anglophonia – French Journal of English Linguistics, 301, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crystal, T. H., & House, A. S. (1988a). Segmental durations in connected-speech signals: Current results. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83(4), 1553–1573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988b). Segmental durations in connected-speech signals: Syllabic stress. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83(4), 1574–1585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. (2019). Intervocalic /s/ voicing in Andean Spanish: Problematizing the assessment of contact-induced change. In G. L. Thompson & S. M. Alvord (Eds.), Contact, community, and connections: Current approaches to Spanish in multilingual populations (pp. 111–146). Wilmington, DE: Vernon Press.Google Scholar
(2021). Intervocalic /s/-voicing in Spanish in contact with Catalan. In E. Núñez (Ed.), Sibilants in Spanish: Diachronic and sociolinguistic analysis (pp. 95–127). Routledge. DOI logo
Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing phonetic features of English, French, German and Spanish. London: Harrap.Google Scholar
Docherty, G. J. (1992). The timing of voicing in British English obstruents. Netherlands Phonetics Archives, 91. Berlin: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eckman, F. R. (1977). Markedness and the contrastive analysis hypothesis. Language Learning, 271, 315–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008). Typological markedness and second language phonology. In H. C. Hansen Edwards & M. L. Zampini (Eds.) Phonology and second language acquisition (pp. 95–115). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Esposito, A. (2002). On vowel height and consonantal voicing effects: Data from Italian. Phonetica, 59(4), 197–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farnetani, E., & Kori, S. (1986). Effects of syllable and word structure on segmental durations in spoken Italian. Speech Communication, 5(1), 17–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Faudree, M. C., & Fujimaki, A. (2020). Exceptions to markedness universals and novel use of Weibull analysis in the acquisition of obstruent voicing contrasts by Japanese speakers of English. Academia Journal of Educational Research, 8(3), 117–137.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–277). York: York Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Bohn, O. S. (2020). The revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In R. Wayland (Ed.), Second language speech learning: Theoretical and empirical progress (pp. 3–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. (1995). Effects of age of second-language learning on the production of English consonants. Speech Communication, 16 (1), 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. (2005). Language universals. The Hague: Mouton De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, M., Savino, M., & Roettger, T. B. (2018). Word final schwa is driven by intonation – The case of Bari Italian. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 143(4), 2474–2486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gros-Bonfiglioli, A. (forthcoming). L’utilisation du linking en anglais : le cas d’apprenants francophones dans IPCE-IPAC. PhD Thesis, University of Paris Nanterre.
Haggard, M. (1978). The devoicing of voiced fricatives. Journal of Phonetics, 6(2), 95–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S. (2010). Phonological features, auditory objects, and illusions. Journal of Phonetics, 38(1), 60–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S., & Nguyen, N. (2004). Influence of syllable-coda voicing on the acoustic properties of syllable-onset/l/in English. Journal of Phonetics, 32(2), 199–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández-Campoy, J. M., & Trudgill, P. (2002). Functional compensation and southern peninsular Spanish /s/ loss. Folia Linguistica Historica, 231, 141–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herry-Bénit, N., Lopez, S., Tennant, J., & Kamiyama, T. (forthcoming). IPCE-IPAC Interphonology of Contemporary English: Methodological issues. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research.
House, A. S. (1961). On vowel duration in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33(9), 1174–1178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
House, A. S., & Fairbanks, G. (1953). The influence of consonant environment upon the secondary acoustical characteristics of vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(1), 105–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hualde, J. I. (2005). The sounds of Spanish with audio CD. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jatteau, A., Vasilescu, I., Lamel, L., & Adda-Decker, M. (2019). Final devoicing of fricatives in French: Studying variation in largescale corpora with automatic alignment. In S. Calhoun, P. Escudero, M. Tabain, & P. Warren (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019 (pp. 295–299). Canberra, Australia: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association Inc.Google Scholar
Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (1997). Syllable structure and the distribution of phonemes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(3), 295–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kisler, T., Reichel, U. D., & Schiel, F. (2017). Multilingual processing of speech via web services. Computer Speech & Language, 451(September), 326–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1976). Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(5), 1208–1221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krämer, M. (2005). Contiguity and non-derived environment blocking of s-voicing in Lombardian and Tuscan Italian. Probus, 17(2), 227–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laeufer, C. (1992). Patterns of voicing-conditioned vowel duration in French and English. Journal of Phonetics, 20(4), 411–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenth, R. (2019). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.3.4. [URL]
Limesurvey Project Team. (2012). Limesurvey: An Open Source Survey Tool. [URL]
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Magno Caldognetto, E., Ferrero, F., Vagges, K., & Bagno, M. (1979). Indici acustici e indici percettivi nel riconoscimento dei suoni linguistici (con applicazione alle consonanti occlusive dell’italiano). Acta Phoniatrica Latina, 21, 219–246.Google Scholar
Mairano, P., Bouzon, C., Capliez, M., & De Iacovo, V. (2019). Acoustic distances, Pillai scores and LDA classification scores as metrics of L2 comprehensibility and nativelikeness. In S. Calhoun, P. Escudero, M. Tabain, & P. Warren (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019 (pp. 1104–1108). Canberra, Australia: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association Inc.Google Scholar
Major, R. C., & Faudree, M. C. (1996). Markedness universals and the acquisition of voicing contrasts by Korean speakers of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 69–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mines, M. A., Hanson, B. F., & Shoup, J. E. (1978). Frequency of occurrence of phonemes in conversational English. Language and Speech, 21(3), 221–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Munro, M. J., & Derwing, T. M. (2006). The functional load principle in ESL pronunciation instruction: An exploratory study. System, 34(4), 520–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nocchi, N., & Filipponio, L. (2012). Lo vuoi co[z]ì o co[s]ì? A sociophonetic study on sibilants in the regional Italian of Livorno (Tuscany). Proceedings of the workshop ‘Sociophonetics, at the crossroads of speech variation, processing and communication’ (pp. 53–56). Pisa: Edizioni della normale.Google Scholar
Nocchi, N., & Schmid, S. (2007). Lenition of voiceless fricatives in two varieties of southern Italian. In J. Trouvain & W. J. Barry (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 1497–1500). Saarbrücken: Institute of Phonetics, Saarland University.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In P. McNeilage (Ed.) The production of speech (pp. 189–216). New York: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Shaughnessy, D. (1981). A study of French vowel and consonant durations. Journal of Phonetics, 9(4), 385–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, G. E., & Lehiste, I. (1960). Duration of syllable nuclei in English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 32(6), 693–703. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pirello, K., Blumstein, S. E., & Kurowski, K. (1997). The characteristics of voicing in syllable-initial fricatives in American English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101(6), 3754–3765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plag, I., Homann, J., & Kunter, G. (2017). Homophony and morphology: The acoustics of word-final S in English. Journal of Linguistics, 53(1), 181–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from [URL]
Raphael, L. J. (1972). Preceding vowel duration as a cue to the perception of the voicing characteristic of word-final consonants in American English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 51(4B), 1296–1303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schielzeth, H. (2010). Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1(2), 103–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, S. (2012). The pronunciation of voiced obstruents in L2 French: A preliminary study of Swiss German learners. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 48(1), 627–659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, S., & Wachter, S. (2015). Le ostruenti sonore nella pronuncia dell’italiano di apprendenti svizzero-tedeschi. In M. Vayra, C. Avesani, & F. Tamburini (Eds.), Il farsi e disfarsi del linguaggio. Acquisizione, mutamento e destrutturazione della struttura sonora del linguaggio/Language acquisition and language loss. Acquisition, change and disorders of the language sound structure. (pp. 203–217). Milano: Officinaventuno Milano.Google Scholar
Smith, C. L. (1997). The devoicing of /z/ in American English: Effects of local and prosodic context. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 471–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, K. N. (1980). Acoustic correlates of some phonetic categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 68(3), 836–842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, K. N., Blumstein, S. E., Glicksman, L., Burton, M., & Kurowski, K. (1992). Acoustic and perceptual characteristics of voicing in fricatives and fricative clusters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(5), 2979–3000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strobl, C., Boulesteix, A. L., Kneib, T., Augustin, T., & Zeileis, A. (2008). Conditional variable importance for random forests. BMC Bioinformatics, 9(1), 307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, S. A., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Models, forests, and trees of York English: Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change, 24(2), 135–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torreira, F., & Ernestus, M. (2012). Weakening of intervocalic /s/ in the Nijmegen Corpus of Casual Spanish. Phonetica, 69(3), 124–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trifu-Dejeu, I. (forthcoming). The interphonology of /r/ in French learners of English in the IPCE-IPAC corpus. PhD dissertation, University of Paris Nanterre.
van Summers, W. (1987). Effects of stress and final-consonant voicing on vowel production: Articulatory and acoustic analyses. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 82(3), 847–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988). F1 structure provides information for final-consonant voicing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84(2), 485–492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vieru-Dimulescu, B., Boula de Mareüil, P., & Adda-Decker, M. (2007). Characterizing non-native French accents using automatic alignement. In J. Trouvain & W. J. Barry (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2217–2220). Saarbrücken: Institute of Phonetics, Saarland University.Google Scholar
Walsh, T., & Parker, F. (1983). Vowel length and vowel transition: Cues to [±voice] in post-vocalic stops. Journal of Phonetics, 11(4), 407–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiltshire, C. (2002). Variation in Spanish aspiration and prosodic boundary constraints. In D. Cresti, T. Satterfield, C. Tortora (Eds.), Current issues in linguistic theory: Selected papers from the XXIXth Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (pp. 375–389). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Mairano, Paolo, Fabián Santiago & Leonardo Contreras Roa
2023. Can L2 Pronunciation Be Evaluated without Reference to a Native Model? Pillai Scores for the Intrinsic Evaluation of L2 Vowels. Languages 8:4  pp. 280 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.