Article published In:
Language, Interaction and Acquisition
Vol. 14:2 (2023) ► pp.218246
References (124)
References
Ahmadian, M. J., & Tavakoli, M. (2011). The effects of simultaneous use of careful online planning and task repetition on accuracy, fluency, and complexity of EFL learners’ oral production. Language Teaching Research, 15 1, 35–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15 (6), 593–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44 1, 169–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andreeva, B., & Dimitrova, S. (2022). Prosodic characteristics of Bulgarian-accented German: Prosodic units and prenuclear pitch accents. Languages, 7 1, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andreeva, B., Demenko, G., Wolska, M., Möbius, B., Zimmerer, F., Jügler, J., & Trouvain, J. (2014). Comparison of pitch range and pitch variation in Slavic and German languages. In N. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th international conference on speech prosody (pp. 776–780). Retrieved from [URL]
Arevart, S., & Nation, P. (1993). Adjusting fluency and grammar through repetition. In J. Oller (Ed.), Methods that work: Ideas for language teachers (pp. 298–308). Heinle & Heinle.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68 1, 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blomgren, M., Chen, Y., Ng, M. L., & Gilbert, H. R. (1998). Acoustic, aerodynamic, physiologic, and perceptual properties of modal and vocal fry registers. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 103 (5), 2649–2658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.37. Retrieved on March 14 2018 from [URL]
Bosker, H., Pinget, A., Quené, H., Sanders, T., & de Jong, N. (2013). What makes speech sound fluent? The contributions of pauses, speed and repairs. Language Testing, 30 1, 159–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bretz, F., Hothorn, T., & Westfall, P. (2010). Multiple comparisons using R. CRC Press.Google Scholar
Bruin, J. (2014). R library: Contrast coding systems for categorical variables. Retrieved from [URL]
Busà, M. G. (2007). New perspectives in teaching pronunciation. In A. Baldry, M. Pavesi, C. Taylor Torsello, & C. Taylor (Eds.), From DIDACTAS to eCoLingua: An ongoing research project on translation and corpus linguistics (pp. 171–188). EUT.Google Scholar
Busà, M. G., & Urbani, M. (2011). A cross linguistic analysis of pitch range in English L1 and L2. In W. S. Lee & E. Zee (Eds.), Proceedings of 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 380–383). Retrieved from [URL]
Butterworth, B. (1975). Hesitation and semantic planning in speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4 1, 75–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bygate, M. (2001). Effects of task repetition on the structure and control of oral language. In M. Bygate, P. Skehan, & M. Swain (Eds.), Researching pedagogic tasks (pp. 23–48). Harlow Longman.Google Scholar
Calbris, G. (2008). From left to right…: Coverbal gestures and their symbolic use of space. In A. Cienki & C. Müller (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 27–53). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cavicchio, F., & Poesio, M. (2009). Multimodal Corpora Annotation: Validation Methods to Assess Coding Scheme Reliability. In M. Kipp, J.-C. Martin, P. Paggio, and D. Heylen (Eds.), Multimodal Corpora LNAI 5509, Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 109–121.Google Scholar
Cienki, A., & Müller, C. (Eds.). (2008). Metaphor and gesture. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Chu, M., Meyer, A., Foulkes, L., & Kita, S. (2014). Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143 1, 694–709. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cravotta, A., Prieto, P., & Busà, M. G. (2021). Exploring the effects of encouraging the use of gestures on speech. Speech Communication, 135 1, 25–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1986). Intonation. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
de Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt’s speaking model adapted. Applied Linguistics, 13 1, 1–24.Google Scholar
De Jong, N. H., Steinel, M. P., Florijn, A. F., Schoonen, R., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2012). Linguistic skills and speaking fluency in a L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 1, 1–24.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. J. (2005). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C. J. Doughty, & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 1–39). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., & Kormos, J. (1998). Problem-solving mechanisms in L2 communication: a psycholinguistic perspective. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20 (3), 349–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: a psychometric Study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 1, 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., & Barkhuizen, G. (2005). Analysing learner language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto, P. (2013). Prosodic structure shapes the temporal realisation of intonation and manual gesture movements. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56 (3), 850–864. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrero, F., Magno Caldognetto, E., & Cosi, P. (1996). Sui piani formantici acustici e uditivi delle vocali di uomo, donna e bambino. In A. Perretti & P. Simonetti (Eds.), Atti del XXIV Convegno Nazionale dell’A.I.A. (pp. 169–178). Arti Grafiche Padovane.Google Scholar
Gao, M. (2021). Comparison of pitch variation and pitch range in L1 and L2 Mandarin Chinese. Poster presented at the 1st International Conference on Tone and Intonation (TAI). Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Gillespie, M., James, A. N., Federmeier, K. D., & Watson, D. G. (2014). Verbal working memory predicts co-speech gesture: Evidence from individual differences. Cognition, 132 1, 174–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gluhareva, D., & Prieto, P. (2017). Training with rhythmic beat gestures favors L2 pronunciation in discourse-demanding situations. Language Teaching Research, 21 (5), 609–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graziano, M., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. (2020). How referential gestures align with speech: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual speakers. Language Learning, 70 1, 266–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M., & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62 (1–2), 51–76.Google Scholar
Gósy, M., Gyarmathy, D., & Beke, A. (2017). Phonetic analysis of filled pauses based on a Hungarian-English learner corpus. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 3 (2), 149–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Götz, S. (2013). Fluency in native and non-native English speech. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. (1998). Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse: A study of learners of French and Swedish. Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P., & Indefrey, P. (2014). The neurobiology of language beyond single words. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37 1, 347–362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herment, S., &, Tortel, A. (2022). The intonation contour of non-finality revisited Implications for EFL teaching. In A. Kirkova-Naskova, A. Henderson, & J. Fouz-González (Eds.), English pronunciation instruction: Research-based insights (pp. 176–195). John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hincks, R. (2005). Measuring liveliness in presentation speech. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2005 (pp. 765–768). Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Hirata, Y., Kelly, S., Huang, J., & Manansala, M. (2014). Effect of hand gestures on auditory learning of second-language vowel length contrasts. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 57 1, 2090–2101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoetjes, M., & van Maastricht, L. (2020). Using gesture to facilitate L2 phoneme acquisition: The importance of gesture and phoneme complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 11 1, 575032. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoetjes, M., Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M. (2014). Does our speech change when we cannot gesture? Speech Communication, 57 1. 257–267.Google Scholar
Holler, J., & Stevens, R. (2007). The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 26 (1), 4–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Housen, A., & Pierrard, M. (2006). Investigating instructed second language acquisition. In A. Housen, & M. Pierrard (Eds.), Investigations in instructed second language acquisition (pp. 12–27). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Indefrey, P. (2011). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 (255), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kearney, E., & Guenther, F. H. (2019). Articulating: The neural mechanisms of speech production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34 (9), 1214–1229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keating, P., & Kuo, G. (2012). Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132 (2), 1050–1060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., McDevitt, T., & Esch, M. (2009). Brief training with co-speech gesture lends a hand to word learning in a foreign language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (2), 313–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. (2000). Hito wa naze jesuchaa o suru noka [why do people gesture?]. Ninchikagaku [Cognitive Studies], 7 (1), 9–21.Google Scholar
Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (1), 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kormos, J. (1999). Monitoring and self-repair in L2. Language Learning, 49 1, 303–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Speech production and L2 acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2011). Speech production and the cognition hypothesis. In P. Robinson (Ed.), L2 task complexity: Researching the cognition hypothesis of language learning and performance (pp. 39–60). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7 1, 54–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. F. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261–283). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kushch, O., & Prieto, P. (2016). The effects of pitch accentuation and beat gestures on information recall in contrastive discourse. In J. Barnes, A. Brugos, S. Shattuck-Hufnagel, & N. Veilleux. Proceedings of the Speech Prosody (pp. 922–925). International Speech Communication Association. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kushch, O., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (2018). Prominence in speech and gesture favour second language novel word learning. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33 (8), 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lazaraton, A. (2004). Gesture and speech in the vocabulary explanations of one ESL teacher: A microanalytic inquiry. Language Learning, 54 (1), 79–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In C. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), Neurocognition of language (pp. 83–122). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lichtman, K. (2021). What about fluency? Implicit vs. explicit training affects artificial mini-language production. Applied Linguistics, 42 (4), 668–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, Y. L. (2021). Gestures as scaffolding for L2 narrative recall: The role of gesture type, task complexity, and working memory. Language Teaching Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, D., & Reed, M. (2021). Exploring the complexity of the L2 intonation system: An acoustic and eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Communication, 6 1, 627316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loehr, D. P. (2012). Temporal, structural, and pragmatic synchrony between intonation and gesture. Laboratory Phonology, 3 (1), 71–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynch, T., & Maclean, J. (2000). Exploring the benefits of task repetition and recycling for classroom language learning. Language Teaching Research, 4 1, 221–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ma, S., & Jin, G. (2022). The relationship between different types of co-speech gestures and L2 speech performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 13 1, 941114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, M. (2014). Bringing back the body into the mind: Gestures enhance word learning in foreign language. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 1, 1467. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, M., Müller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate. Human Brain Mapping, 32 1, 982–998. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, M., & von Kriegstein, K. (2012). Gestures enhance foreign language learning. Biolinguistics, 6 1, 393–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCafferty, S. G. (2002). Gesture and creating zones of proximal development for second language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 86 (2), 192–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClave, E. (1998). Pitch and manual gestures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27 1, 69–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2005). Gesture and thought. University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mennen, I. (2007). Phonological and phonetic influences in non-native intonation. In J. Trouvain, & U. Gut (Eds.), Non-native prosody. Phonetic description and teaching practice (pp. 53–76). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., & Docherty, G. (2008). A methodological study into the linguistic dimensions of pitch range differences between German and English. In P. L. Barbosa, S. Madureira, & C. Reis. Proceedings of the IV Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 527–530). ISCA Archive [URL]
Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., Watt, N., & Miller, N. (2008). An autosegmental-metrical investigation of intonation in people with Parkinson’s Disease. Asia-Pacific Journal of Speech Language and Hearing, 11 (4), 205–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., Docherty, G. (2012). Cross-language differences in fundamental frequency range: A comparison of English and German. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131 (3), 2249–2260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mittelberg, I., & Waugh, L. R. (2009). Metonymy first, metaphor second: A cognitive-semiotic approach to multimodal figures of thought in co-speech gesture. In C. Forceville & E. Urios-Aparisi (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 329–356). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Morett, L. M., & Chang, L. Y. (2014). Emphasising sound and meaning: Pitch gestures enhance Mandarin lexical tone acquisition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 98 (3), 773–794.Google Scholar
Morett, L. M., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2019). Listeners consider alternative speaker productions in discourse comprehension and memory: Evidence from beat gesture and pitch accenting. Memory and Cognition, 47 1, 1515–1530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mori, J., & Hayashi, M. (2006). The achievement of intersubjectivity through embodied completions: A study of interactions between first and second language speakers. Applied Linguistics, 27 1, 195–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Shaughnessy, M. (1980). Gestures: Their origins and distribution. New York.Google Scholar
Morsella, E., & Krauss, R. M. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 117 1, 411–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nergis, A. (2021). Can explicit instruction of formulaic sequences enhance L2 oral fluency? Lingua, 255 1, 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, A. T., & Dao, D. M. (2018). The acquisition of question intonation by Vietnamese learners of English. Asian Journal of Second Foreign Language Education, 3 (3), 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. (2008). A fresh look at the foundations of mathematics: Gesture and the psychological reality of conceptual metaphor. In A. Cienki & C. Müller (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 93–114). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pang, F., & Skehan, P. (2014). Self-reported planning behaviour and L2 performance in narrative retelling. In P. Skehan (Ed.), Processing perspectives on task performance (pp. 95–128). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Patterson, D. (2000). A linguistic approach to pitch range modelling. University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Peters, J., Frank, M., & Rohloff, M. (2020). Pitch range variation in High German (L1) and Low German (L2). In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 650–654). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. F. (2006). A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9 1, 319–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, K. J., Bird, H., & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10 1, 747–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core Team. (2020). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–149. Retrieved from [URL]
Poesio, M., & Artstein, R. (2008). Introduction to the special issue on ambiguity and semantic judgments. Research on Language and Computation, 6 1, 241–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quinn-Allen, L. Q. (1995). The effects of emblematic gestures on the development and access of mental representations of French expressions. The Modern Language Journal, 79 1, 521–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M. & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7 (4), 226–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (1996). Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule-search, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18 (1), 27–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2007). Increasing retention without increasing study time. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16 (4), 183–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rousse-Malpat, A., Koote, L., Steinkrauss, R., & Verspoor, M. (2021). Parlez-vous francais? Effects of structure-based versus dynamic-usage-based approaches on oral proficiency. Language Teaching Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santiago, F., & Delais-Roussarie, E. (2014). What explains the distribution and form of non-final rising contours in French as an L2? In Concordia Working Papers in Applied Linguistics Vol. 5 (pp. 561–579). Retrieved from [URL]
Schmidt, R. (2000). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3–32). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwab, S., & Goldman, J. P. (2016). Do speakers show different F when they speak in different languages? The case of English, French and German. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2016. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Segalowitz, N. (2010). Cognitive bases of L2 fluency. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Second language fluency and its underlying cognitive and social determinants. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 54 (2), 79–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seyfeddinipur, M. (2006). Disfluency: Interrupting speech and gesture. Doctoral thesis. Radboud University.
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Yasinnik, Y., Veilleux, N., & Renwick, M. (2007). A method for studying the time alignment of gestures and prosody in American English: ‘Hits’ and pitch accents in academic-lecture-style speech. In A. Esposito, M. Bratanic, E. Keller & M. Marinaro (Eds.), Fundamentals of verbal and nonverbal communication and the biometric issue (pp. 34–44). IOS Press.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2009). Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis. Applied Linguistics, 30 (4), 510–532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Processing perspectives on task performance. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P., & Foster, P. (2001). Cognition and tasks. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 183–205). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sloetjes, H., & Wittenburg, P. (2008). Annotation by category – ELAN and ISO DCR. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, M. Bente, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, S. Piperidis, & D. Tapias (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. Retrieved from [URL]
Smotrova, T., & Lantolf, J. P. (2013). The function of gesture in lexically focused L2 instructional conversations. Modern Language Journal, 97 1, 397–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sueyoshi, A., & Hardison, D. M. (2005). The role of gestures and facial cues in second language listening comprehension. Language Learning, 55 (4), 661–699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taguchi, N. (2008). Building language blocks in L2 Japanese: Chunk learning and the development of complexity and fluency in spoken production. Foreign Language Annals, 41 (1), 132–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tavakoli, P., & Skehan, P. (2005). Strategic planning, task structure, and performance testing. In R. Ellis (Ed.), Planning and task performance in a second language. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tellier, M. (2008). The effect of gestures on second language memorisation by young children. Gesture, 8 (2), 219–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trouvain, J., Zimmerer, F., Möbius, B., Gósy, M., & Bonneau, A. (2017). Segmental, prosodic and fluency features in phonetic learner corpora: Introduction to the special issue. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 3 (2), 105–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullakanoja, R. (2007). Comparison of pitch range in Finnish (L1) and Russian (L2). In J. Trouvain, & W. J. Barry (Eds), Proceedings of the 16th International congress of phonetic sciences (pp. 1701–1704). Retrieved from [URL]
Volin, J., Poesova, K., & Weingartova, L. (2015). Speech melody properties in English, Czech and Czech English: Reference and interference. Research in Language, 13 (1), 107–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, L., & Chu, M. (2013). The role of beat gesture and pitch accent in semantic processing: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 51 (13), 2847–2855. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Z. (2014). Online time pressure manipulations: L2 speaking performance under five types of planning and repetition conditions. In P. Skehan (Ed.), Task-based language teaching: Processing perspectives on task performance (p. 27–61). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yasinnik, Y., Renwick, M., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2004). The timing of speech-accompanying gestures with respect to gestures. In Proceedings of from sound to sense (pp. 97–102). Retrieved from [URL]
Zimmerer, F., Jügler, J., Andreeva, B., Möbius, B., & Trouvain, J. (2014). Too cautious to vary more? A comparison of pitch variation in native and non-native productions of French and German speakers. In N. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th international conference on speech prosody (pp. 1037–1041). Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo