References
Anderson, J., Morgan, J.L. & White, K.S
(2003) A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech 461, 155–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B
(2010) Review of I. Gülzow & N. Gargarina: ‘‘Frequency effects in language acquisition: defining the limits of frequency as an explanatory concept’’. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Journal of Child Language 37(2), 453–460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behrens, H
(2006) The input–output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes 21(1/2/3), 2–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, A.A
(2005) Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: relationships with input properties and adjectival semantic typology. Journal of Child Language 321, 535–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M.H., Cote, L.R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L. & Vyt, A
(2004) Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development 75(4), 1115–1139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broen P
1972The verbal environment of the language-hearing child. Monograph of American Speech and Hearing Association 171.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U.E
(1964) Three processes in the child’s acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34(2), 133–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., Cazden, C. & Bellugi, U
(1969) The child’s grammar from I to III. In Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 21, 28–73). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R., Salerno, R.A. & Sachs, J
(1972) Some characteristics of adults’ speech to children. University of Connecticut, Language Acquisition Laboratory, Report 6.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M
(2003) A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science 27(6), 843–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S., & Gopnik, A
(1995) Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 22(03), 497–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N
(1965) Aspects of a theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1980) Rules and representations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de León, L
(1998) The emergent participant: patterns of socialization of Tzotzil (Mayan) infants. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 8(2), 131–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demetras, M.J., Post, K.N. & Snow, C.E
(1986) Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language 13(2), 275–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drach, K
(1969) The language of the parent: a pilot study. Working Paper Number 4: The Structure of Linguistic Input to Children . University of California, Language Behavior Research Laboratory, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Elman, J.L., Bates, E.A., Johnson, M.H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K
(1996) Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Garfield, J.L
(1994) Innateness. In S. Guttenplan (ed.), A companion to the philosophy of mind (366–374). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B.J
(1994) Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garnica, O
(1977) Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C.E. & Ferguson, C.A. (eds.), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (63–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D
(1982) Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S.A. Kuczaj (ed.), Language development: language, thought, and culture (301–334). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goodman, J., Dale, P. & Li, P
(2008) Does frequency count? Journal of Child Language 35(3), 515. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., Choi, S., & Baumberger, T
(1996) Cross-linguistic differences in early semantic and cognitive development. Cognitive Development 11(2), 197–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, R
(2005) Ethnologue: Languages of the World. Dallas: SIL International.Google Scholar
Gülzow, I. & Gagarina, N
(eds.) (2007) Frequency effects in language acquisition: defining the limits of frequency as an explanatory concept. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hadley, P., Rispoli, M., Fitzgerald, C., & Bahnsen, A
(2011) Predictors of morphosyntactic growth in typically developing toddlers: contributions of parent input and child sex. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 54(2), 549–566. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haegeman, L
(1994) Introduction to government and binding theory (2nd edition). Oxford, U.K. and Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T.R
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L
(2002) How children use input to acquire a lexicon? Child Development 73(2), 418–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, M
(1983) L’acquisition Du Langage Repensée : Les Recherches Interlangues. L’Année Psychologique 83(1), 225–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legate, J.A. & Yang C
(2007) Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition 14(3), 315–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E.V
(2010) Input and first language acquisition. Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua 1201, 2546–2556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C
(1985) The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language 12(2), 271–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moerk, E.L
(1975) Verbal interactions between children and their mothers during the preschool years. Developmental Psychology 11(6), 788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan, J.L
(1996) A rhythmic bias in preverbal speech segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language 351, 666–688. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K
(1973)  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 149, 1–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newport, E.L
(1975) Motherese. The speech of mothers to young children. Technical Report n 521, Dissertation Abstracts International, Ann Arbor, MI 1975, 36, 2503B 04B, Center for Human Information Processing, San Diego, University of California.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L.R
(1977) Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C.E. Snow & C.A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (109–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B
(1995) The impact of language socialization on grammatical development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (eds.), The handbook of child language (73–94). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Pye, C., Ingram, D. & List, H
(1987) A comparison of initial consonant acquisition in English and Quiché. In K. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (eds.), Children’s Language (Vol. 61, 175–190). Hillsdale, N.J.: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Pfuderer, C
(1969) Some suggestions for a syntactic characterization of baby talk style. Working paper n 14, Berkeley: University of California.Google Scholar
Phillips, J.R
(1973) Syntax and vocabulary of mother’s speech to young children. Age and sex comparisons. Child Development 441, 182–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pullum, G.K. & Scholz, B.C
(2002) Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 191, 9–50.Google Scholar
Ramsey, W. & Stich, S.P
(1991) Connectionism and three levels of nativism. In W. Ramsey, S.P. Stich & D.E. Rumelhart (eds.), Philosophy and connectionist theory (287–310). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Remick, H
(1976) Maternal speech to children during language acquisition. In W. Von Raffler-Engel & Y. Lebrun (eds.), Baby talk and infant speech (223–233).Amsterdam: Swets; Zetilinger B.V.Google Scholar
Roy, B.C. & Frank, M.C. & Roy, D
(2009). Exploring word learning in a high-density longitudinal corpus. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society . Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Seidenberg, M.S
(1997) Language acquisition and use: learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science 2751, 1599–1603. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shatz M. & Gelman, R
(1973) The development of communication skills: modification in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (5, Serial No. 152). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.I
(1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition 21, 1157–1256.Google Scholar
Snow, C.E
(1972) Mother’s speech to children learning language. Child Development 431, 549–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1977) The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4(1), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snow, C.E., & Ferguson, C.A
(1977) Talking to children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tardif, T
(1996) Nouns are not always learned before verbs: evidence from Mandarin speakers’ early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32(3), 492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A.L., Lieven, E.V., Pine, J.M. & Rowland, C.F
(2004) Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language 31(1), 61–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M
(2005) Beyond formalities: the case of language acquisition. The Linguistic Review 22(2-4), 183–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V
(1999) Input and language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of child language acquisition (497–530). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vorster, J
(1974) Mothers’ speech to children: some methodological considerations. Universiteit van Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Zamuner, T.S., Gerken, L. & Hammond, M
(2005) The acquisition of phonology based on input: a closer look at the relation of cross-linguistic and child language data. Lingua 115(10), 1403–1426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Garrote Salazar, Marta
2023. The role of the input frequency in L1 Spanish phonological acquisition. A corpus-based study. Loquens 9:1-2  pp. e089 ff. DOI logo
Heurdier, Julien
2018. Types syntaxiques des énoncés de jeunes enfants en dialogue avec le père ou la mère. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 9:2  pp. 137 ff. DOI logo
MITROFANOVA, Natalia & Marit WESTERGAARD
2018. Acquisition of locative utterances in Norwegian: structure-building via lexical learning. Journal of Child Language 45:4  pp. 981 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.