The role of input in early first language morphosyntactic development
Sophie Kern | Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage (UMR 5596 CNRS & Université de Lyon)
References (58)
Anderson, J., Morgan, J.L. & White, K.S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech 461, 155–182.
Ambridge, B. (2010). Review of I. Gülzow & N. Gargarina: ‘‘Frequency effects in language acquisition: defining the limits of frequency as an explanatory concept’’. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Journal of Child Language 37(2), 453–460.
Behrens, H. (2006). The input–output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes 21(1/2/3), 2–24.
Blackwell, A.A. (2005). Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: relationships with input properties and adjectival semantic typology. Journal of Child Language 321, 535–562.
Bornstein, M.H., Cote, L.R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L. & Vyt, A. (2004). Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development 75(4), 1115–1139.
Broen P., 1972. The verbal environment of the language-hearing child. Monograph of American Speech and Hearing Association 171.
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U.E. (1964). Three processes in the child’s acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34(2), 133–151.
Brown, R., Cazden, C. & Bellugi, U. (1969). The child’s grammar from I to III. In Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 21, 28–73). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Brown, R., Salerno, R.A. & Sachs, J. (1972). Some characteristics of adults’ speech to children. University of Connecticut, Language Acquisition Laboratory, Report 6.
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science 27(6), 843–873.
Choi, S., & Gopnik, A. (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 22(03), 497–529.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of a theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
de León, L. (1998). The emergent participant: patterns of socialization of Tzotzil (Mayan) infants. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 8(2), 131–161.
Demetras, M.J., Post, K.N. & Snow, C.E. (1986). Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language 13(2), 275–292.
Drach, K. (1969). The language of the parent: a pilot study.
Working Paper Number 4: The Structure of Linguistic Input to Children
. University of California, Language Behavior Research Laboratory, Berkeley.
Elman, J.L., Bates, E.A., Johnson, M.H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Garfield, J.L. (1994). Innateness. In S. Guttenplan (ed.), A companion to the philosophy of mind (366–374). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B.J. (1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Garnica, O. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C.E. & Ferguson, C.A. (eds.), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (63–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S.A. Kuczaj (ed.), Language development: language, thought, and culture (301–334). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
Goodman, J., Dale, P. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Journal of Child Language 35(3), 515.
Gopnik, A., Choi, S., & Baumberger, T. (1996). Cross-linguistic differences in early semantic and cognitive development. Cognitive Development 11(2), 197–225.
Gordon, R. (2005). Ethnologue: Languages of the World. Dallas: SIL International.
Gülzow, I. & Gagarina, N. (eds.) (2007). Frequency effects in language acquisition: defining the limits of frequency as an explanatory concept. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Hadley, P., Rispoli, M., Fitzgerald, C., & Bahnsen, A. (2011). Predictors of morphosyntactic growth in typically developing toddlers: contributions of parent input and child sex. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 54(2), 549–566.
Haegeman, L. (1994). Introduction to government and binding theory (2nd edition). Oxford, U.K. and Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Hart, B. & Risley, T.R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon? Child Development 73(2), 418–433.
Kail, M. (1983). L’acquisition Du Langage Repensée : Les Recherches Interlangues. L’Année Psychologique 83(1), 225–258.
Legate, J.A. & Yang C. (2007). Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition 14(3), 315–344.
Lieven, E.V. (2010). Input and first language acquisition. Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua 1201, 2546–2556.
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language 12(2), 271–295.
Moerk, E.L. (1975). Verbal interactions between children and their mothers during the preschool years. Developmental Psychology 11(6), 788.
Morgan, J.L. (1996). A rhythmic bias in preverbal speech segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language 351, 666–688.
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 149, 1–135.
Newport, E.L. (1975). Motherese. The speech of mothers to young children. Technical Report n 521, Dissertation Abstracts International, Ann Arbor, MI 1975, 36, 2503B 04B, Center for Human Information Processing, San Diego, University of California.
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L.R. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C.E. Snow & C.A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (109–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1995). The impact of language socialization on grammatical development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (eds.), The handbook of child language (73–94). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Pye, C., Ingram, D. & List, H. (1987). A comparison of initial consonant acquisition in English and Quiché. In K. Nelson & A. van Kleeck (eds.), Children’s Language (Vol. 61, 175–190). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum
Pfuderer, C. (1969). Some suggestions for a syntactic characterization of baby talk style. Working paper n 14, Berkeley: University of California.
Phillips, J.R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mother’s speech to young children. Age and sex comparisons. Child Development 441, 182–185.
Pullum, G.K. & Scholz, B.C. (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 191, 9–50.
Ramsey, W. & Stich, S.P. (1991). Connectionism and three levels of nativism. In W. Ramsey, S.P. Stich & D.E. Rumelhart (eds.), Philosophy and connectionist theory (287–310). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Remick, H. (1976). Maternal speech to children during language acquisition. In W. Von Raffler-Engel & Y. Lebrun (eds.), Baby talk and infant speech (223–233).Amsterdam: Swets; Zetilinger B.V.
Roy, B.C. & Frank, M.C. & Roy, D. (2009). Exploring word learning in a high-density longitudinal corpus. In
Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
. Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Seidenberg, M.S. (1997). Language acquisition and use: learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science 2751, 1599–1603.
Shatz M. & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: modification in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (5, Serial No. 152).
Slobin, D.I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition 21, 1157–1256.
Snow, C.E. (1972). Mother’s speech to children learning language. Child Development 431, 549–565.
Snow, C.E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4(1), 1–22.
Snow, C.E., & Ferguson, C.A. (1977). Talking to children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: evidence from Mandarin speakers’ early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32(3), 492.
Theakston, A.L., Lieven, E.V., Pine, J.M. & Rowland, C.F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language 31(1), 61–100.
Tomasello, M. (2005). Beyond formalities: the case of language acquisition. The Linguistic Review 22(2-4), 183–197.
Valian, V. (1999). Input and language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of child language acquisition (497–530). New York: Academic Press.
Vorster, J. (1974). Mothers’ speech to children: some methodological considerations. Universiteit van Amsterdam.
Zamuner, T.S., Gerken, L. & Hammond, M. (2005). The acquisition of phonology based on input: a closer look at the relation of cross-linguistic and child language data. Lingua 115(10), 1403–1426.
Cited by (3)
Cited by three other publications
Garrote Salazar, Marta
2023.
The role of the input frequency in L1 Spanish phonological acquisition. A corpus-based study.
Loquens 9:1-2
► pp. e089 ff.
MITROFANOVA, Natalia & Marit WESTERGAARD
2018.
Acquisition of locative utterances in Norwegian: structure-building via lexical learning.
Journal of Child Language 45:4
► pp. 981 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.