References (37)
Antinucci, F. & Miller, R. (1976). How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language 31, 167–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Applebee, A.N. (1978). The child’s concept of story: ages two to seventeen. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (2001). Cognitive-conceptual development and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes: the development of time concepts and verb tense. In M. Bowerman & S.C. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (450–474). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. & Slobin, D.I. (1994). Different ways of relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1986). First steps in acquiring conditionals. In E. Traugott, A. ter Meulen, J.S. Reilly & C.A. Ferguson (eds.), On conditionals (285–307). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bronckart, J.-P. & Sinclair, H. (1973). Time, tense and aspect. Cognition 21, 107–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of verbal behavior by B.F. Skinner. Language 351, 26–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M.M. & Clark, E.V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language 30(3), 637–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dendale, P. & Tasmowski, L. (éds.) (2001). Le conditionnel en français. Université de Metz: Recherches linguistiques 25.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1971). Les relations temporelles dans le langage de l’enfant. Genève: Droz.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1988). The mechanisms of “Construction Grammar”. Berkeley Linguistics Society 141, 35–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D., Grondelaers, S. & Bakema, P. (1994). The structure of lexical variation: Meaning, naming, and context. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: aconstruction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A.N. & Kuhl P.K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: minds, brains and how children learn. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Halliday, M.A.K. (1975). Learning how to Mean: explorations in the development of language. New York: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M. (2003). Children’s discourse: person, time, and space across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaper, W. (1980). The use of the past tense in games of pretend. Journal of Child Language, 71, 213–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar (Vol. 11). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Leroy, M., Morgenstern, A. & Caët, S. (2010). «L’auto-rectification chez l’enfant: appropriation et internalisation». dans M. Candea & R. Mir-Samii (editeurs), La rectification à l’oral et à l’écrit, ouvrage en hommage à Mary-Annick Morel (221–234). Paris: Ophrys.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. (2010). Input and first language acquisition: evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua 1201, 2546–2556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michaelis, L.A. (2006). Construction grammar. In K. Brown (ed.), The encyclopedia of language and linguistics (Second edition, Vol. 31, 73–84). Oxford: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C. (2012). The Paris corpus. Journal of French language studies 221, 7–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Sekali, M. (2009). What can child language tell us about prepositions? A contrastive corpus-based study of cognitive and social-pragmatic factors. dans J. Zlatev, M. Johansson Falck, C. Lundmark & M. Andrén (eds.), Studies in language and cognition (261–275). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Parisse, C. & Sekali, M. (2010). A la source du futur: premières formes verbales dans les productions spontanées de deux enfants français de 18 mois à 3 ans. Faits de Langues: Le futur 331, 163–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parisse, C. & Le Normand, M.-T. (2006). Une méthode pour évaluer la production du langage spontané chez l’enfant de 2 à 4 ans. Glossa 971, 20–41.Google Scholar
Parisse, C. & Morgenstern, A. (2012). The unfolding of the verbal temporal system in French children’s speech between 18 and 36 months. Journal of French language studies 22(1), 95–114 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M. & Yarlett, D. (2008). Linguistic self-correction in the absence of feedback: a new approach to the logical problem of language acquisition. Cognitive Science 311, 927–960. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reichenbach, H. (1947). Elements of symbolic logic. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Reilly, J.S. (1986). The acquisition of temporals and conditionals. In E.C. Traugott, A. ter Meulen, J.S. Reilly & C.A. Ferguson (eds.), On conditionals (300–331). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sabeau-Jouannet, E. (1977). L’expression de l’organisation spatiale et temporelle, son évolution chez l’enfant de 2 à 5 ans. In F. François, D. François, E. Sabeau-Jouannet & M. Sourdot (eds.), La syntaxe de l’enfant avant 5 ans (193–204). Paris: Larousse.Google Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2007). Entrenchment, salience, and basic levels. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (117–138). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D.I. Slobin (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 21: Theoretical Issues (1157–1256). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, C.S. (1980). The acquisition of time talk: relation between child and adult grammar. Journal of Child Language 71, 263–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language, a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Weist, R.M. (2002). The first language acquisition of tense and aspect: a review. In R. Salaberry & Y. Shirai (eds.), The L2 acquisition of tense-aspect morphology (21–78). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weist, R.M., Wysocka, H., Witkowska-Stadnik, K., Buczowska, W. & Konieczna, E. (1984). The defective tense hypothesis: on the emergence of tense and aspect in child polish. Journal of Child Language 111, 347–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (3)

Cited by three other publications

Canut, Emmanuelle, Morgane Jourdain & Christine Bocéréan
2023. Developmental Markers of Pre-schoolers’ Temporal and Causal Connectivity in Two Discourse Contexts: Data from the French Language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 52:6  pp. 2373 ff. DOI logo
Parisse, Christophe, Sophie de Pontonx & Aliyah Morgenstern
2018. L’imparfait dans le langage de l’enfant. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 9:2  pp. 183 ff. DOI logo
Parisse, Christophe, Sophie de Pontonx & Aliyah Morgenstern
2017. Développement et stabilisation de l’expression de la temporalité. In Le langage de l’enfant,  pp. 148 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.