References
Bedore, L.M. & Leonard, L.B
(2001) Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 441, 905–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 261, 195–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R.A. & Slobin, D.I
(1994) Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, E., Paradis, J. & Sorenson Duncan, T
(2012) Effects of input properties, vocabulary size and L1 on the development of third person singular -s in child L2 English. Language Learning 62(3), 965–994. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bronckart, J.P. & Sinclair, H
(1973) Time, tense and aspect. Cognition 2(1), 107–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M
(1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 631(4, Serial No. 255). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cortés M. & Vila, L
(1991) Uso y función de las formas temporales en el habla infantil. Infancia y Aprendizaje 531, 17–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, A.R
(1985) Learning to describe past experiences in conversation. Discourse Processes 8(2), 177–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V.A., Weisleder, A
(2013) SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 month. Developmental Science 16(2), 234–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
French, L.A. & Nelson, K
(1985) Young children’s knowledge of relational terms. New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V.C
(1977) A study of the comings and goings of the speakers of four languages: Spanish, Japanese, English, and Turkish. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 21, 61–94.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V.C., Sebastián, E. & Soto, P
(1999) The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge? International Journal of Bilingualism 31, 133–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V.F., Restrepo, M.A., Bedore, L., Peña, E. & Anderson, R
(2000) Language sample analysis in Spanish-speaking children: methodological considerations. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 311, 88–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T.R
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hernández Pina, F
(1984) Teorías Psicolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M
(2002) Children’s discourse: person, space and time across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E
(2006) Environmental supports for language acquisition. In D.K. Dickinson & S.B. Neuman (eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 21, 163–172). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E
(1998) The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics 19(4), 603–629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J.A
(2002) Do you know what we’re going to do this summer? Mother’s talk to young children about future events. Journal of Cognition and Development 31, 49–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) The development of future time concepts through mother - child conversation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 521, 70–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V.A. & Fernald, A
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science 111, 31–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T
(1991) Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 236–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado D. & Maldonado R
(2001) Determinaciones semánticas de la flexión verbal en la adquisición temprana del español. In C. Rojas Nieto & L. de León Pasquel (eds.), La adquisición de la lengua materna: español, lenguas mayas, eusquera (165–200). México, D.F.: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.Google Scholar
Kern, S. & Dos Santos, C
(2011) Input et acquisition du lexique en français: rôle de la fréquence et de la densité de voisinage. Travaux de didactique du français langue étrangère 65/661, 53–70.Google Scholar
Küntay, A. & Slobin, D.I
(2002) Putting interaction back into child language: examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication 61, 5–14.Google Scholar
Labov, W
(1972) The transformation of experience in narrative discourse. In W. Labov (ed.), Language in the inner city: studies on the black English vernacular (354–396). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Some further steps in narrative analysis. Journal of Narrative and Life History 71, 395–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. & Nelson K
(1994) Words in discourse: a dialectical approach to the acquisition of meaning and use. Journal of Child Language 211, 367–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E
(2010) Input and first language acquisition: evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua 1201, 2546–2556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lubbers-Quesada M
(2006) L2 Acquisition of temporal reference in Spanish and the interaction of adverbials, morphology and clause structure. In N. Sagarra & A.M. Toribio (eds.), Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistic Symposium (157–168). Sommerville, M.A.: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B
(2000) The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk (third edition). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mayer, M
(1969) Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K
(1989) The linguistic construction of self in time. In K. Nelson (eds.), Narratives from the crib (284–308). Cambridge, M.A.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1996) Language in cognitive development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Young minds in social worlds: experience, meaning and memory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosemberg, C.R., Silva, M.L. & Stein, A
(2008) El uso de conectores causales por parte de niños pequeños en situaciones de elaboración de narrativas en colaboración. Poster presented at Language, Communication and Cognition . Brighton University, Brighton, England, August 4th-7th 2008.
(2011) Narrativas infantiles en contexto: un estudio en hogares de barrios urbano marginados de Buenos Aires. Revista del Instituto de Investigaciones en Ciencias de la Educación 281, 135–154.Google Scholar
Sachs, J
(1983) Talking about there and then: the emergence of displaced references in parent-children discourse. In K.E. Nelson (ed.), Children`s language. (Vol. 41, 1–28). New York: Gardener Press.Google Scholar
Sanz-Torrent, M
(2002) Los verbos en niños con Trastorno del Lenguaje. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología XXII1, 100–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sebastián, E. & Slobin, D
(1994) Development of linguistic forms: Spanish. In R. Berman & D. Slobin (eds.), Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study (157–186). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J. & Clouthard, R.M
(1975) Towards the analysis of discourse: the English used by teachers and pupils. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I. & Bocaz, A
(1988) Learning to talk about movement though time and space: the development of narrative abilities in Spanish and English. Lenguas Modernas 151, 5–24.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M
(2003) Constructing a language: a usage – based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2009) The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In E.L. Bavin (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (69–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Mannle, S. & Kruger, A
(1986) Linguistic environment of one to two year old twins. Development Psychology 221, 169–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Todd, J
(1983) Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language 41, 197–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uccelli, P
(2009) Emerging temporality: past tense and temporal/aspectual markers in Spanish speaking children’s intra-conversational narratives. Journal of Child Language 361, 929–966. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C
(2010) The acquisition of early verbs in French: assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed speech. First Language 30(3-4), 287–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A. & Fernald, A
(2011) Variation in early language experiences influences processing and language growth. 24th Annual Cuny Conference on Human Sentence Processing . Stanford University, Palo Alto, United States, March 24-26th 2011.
Weizman, Z.O. & Snow, C
(2001) Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology 171, 265–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Li, Hui, Luyao Liang & Dandan Wu
2022. Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study. Languages 7:2  pp. 133 ff. DOI logo
Migdalek, Maia J. & Celia R. Rosemberg
2020. SES differences in children’s argumentative production. Europe’s Journal of Psychology 16:2  pp. 193 ff. DOI logo
Tobia, Valentina, Taylor Landis & Paulo Graziano
2021. Examining Temporal Cognition in Preschoolers With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Insights From Parent–Child Interactions. Journal of Child and Family Studies 30:9  pp. 2315 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.