Article published In:
Languages in Contrast
Vol. 22:1 (2022) ► pp.136159
References (43)
References
Athanasopoulos, P. and Bylund, E. 2012. Does Grammatical Aspect Affect Motion Event Cognition? A Cross-Linguistic Comparison of English and Swedish Speakers. Cognitive Science 37(2): 286–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S. 2000. Irony as Relevant Inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics 32(6): 793–826. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bepperling, S. and Härtl, H. 2013. Ereigniskonzeptualisierung im Zweitspracherwerb ― Thinking for Speaking im Vergleich von Muttersprachlern und Lernern. Zeitschrift für Semiotik 35(1–2): 159–191.Google Scholar
Bernabeu, P. and Tillman, R. 2019. More Refined Typology and Design in Linguistic Relativity: The Case of Motion Event Encoding. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics 8(2): 163–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E., Athanasopoulos, P. and Oostendorp, M. 2013. Motion Event Cognition and Grammatical Aspect: Evidence from Afrikaans. Linguistics 51(5): 929–955. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cardini, F. 2010. Evidence against Whorfian Effects in Motion Conceptualisation. Journal of Pragmatics 421: 1442–1459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, T., Xu, M., Tu, J., Wang, H. and Niu, X. 2018. Relationship between Omnibus and Post-hoc Tests: An Investigation of Performance of the F Test in ANOVA. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry 30(1): 60–64.Google Scholar
Do, M. L., Papafragou, A. and Trueswell, J. 2020. Cognitive and Pragmatic Factors in Language Production: Evidence from Source-Goal Motion Events. Cognition 2051: 1–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engemann, H., Hendriks, H., Hickmann, M., Soroli, E. and Vincent, C. 2015. How Language Impacts Memory of Motion Events in English and French. Cognitive Processing 16(1): 209–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feinmann, D. 2019. Language and Thought in the Motion Domain: Methodological Considerations and New Empirical Evidence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 49(1): 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filipović, L. 2009. Motion Events in Eyewitness Interviews, Translation and Memory: Typological and Psycholinguistic Perspectives. Language and Linguistics Compass 3(1): 300–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011. Speaking and Remembering in one or two Languages: Bilingual vs. Monolingual Lexicalization and Memory for Motion Events. International Journal of Bilingualism 15(4): 466–485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filipović, L. and Geva, S. 2012. Language-Specific Effects on Lexicalization and Memory of Motion Events. In Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Language, Culture, and Cognition, L. Filipović and K. M. Jaszczolt (eds), 269–282. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flecken, M. (ed.). 2010. Event Conceptualization in Language Production of Early Bilinguals. Nijmegen: Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Flecken, M., Carroll, M., Weimar, K. and von Stutterheim, C. 2015. Driving along the Road or Heading for the Village? Conceptual Differences Underlying Motion Event Encoding in French, German, and French–German L2 Users. The Modern Language Journal 99(S1): 100–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flecken, M., von Stutterheim, C. and Carroll, M. 2014. Grammatical Aspect Influences Motion Event Perception: Evidence from a Cross-Linguistic Non-Verbal Recognition Task. Language and Cognition 6(1): 45–78.Google Scholar
Georgakopoulos, T. and Härtl, H. 2020. Goal Prevalence and Situation Types: An Empirical Analysis of Differences in Greek and German Motion Event Descriptions. In Theoretical Approaches to Contrastive Linguistics. Morphological and Syntactic Perspectives, M. Georgiafentis, G. Giannoulopoulou, M. Koliopoulou and A. Tsokoglou (eds), 262–280. London: Bloomsbury Academic. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Georgakopoulos, T., Härtl, H. and Sioupi, A. 2019. Goal Realization: An Empirically Based Comparison between English, German and Greek. Languages in Contrast 19(2): 280–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z. and Bock, K. 2000. What the Eyes Say about Speaking. Psychological Science 111: 274–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, J. C. (ed.). 1996. Multiple Comparisons: Theory and Methods. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Johanson, M. and Papafragou, A. 2010. Universality and Language Specificity in the Acquisition of Path Vocabulary. Paper presented at the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD 34). Boston University, USA, 6–8 November 2009.Google Scholar
Lakusta, L. and DiFabrizio, S. 2017. And, the Winner is… A Visual Preference for Endpoints over Starting Points in Infants’ Motion Event Representations. Infancy 22(3): 323–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakusta, L. and Landau, B. 2012. Language and Memory for Motion Events: Origins of the Asymmetry between Source and Goal Paths. Cognitive Science 36(3): 517–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005. Starting at the End: The Importance of Goals in Spatial Language. Cognition 96(1): 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakusta, L., Wagner, L., O’Hearn, K. and Landau, B. 2007. Conceptual Foundations of Spatial Language: Evidence for a Goal Bias in Infants. Language Learning and Development 3(3): 179–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (ed.). 2008. Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liao, Y., Flecken, M., Dijkstra, K. and Zwaan, R. A. 2019. Going Places in Dutch and Mandarin Chinese: Conceptualising the Path of Motion Cross-Linguistically. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 35(4): 498–520. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mertins, B. (ed.). 2018. Sprache und Kognition: Ereigniskonzeptualisierung im Deutschen und Tschechischen. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S. and Van der Meulen, F. F. 2000. Phonological Priming Effects on Speech Onset Latencies and Viewing Times in Object Naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 7(2): 314–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., Massey, C. and Gleitman, L. 2002. Shake, Rattle, ‘n’ Roll: The Representation of Motion in Language and Cognition. Cognition 84(2): 189–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Purschke, C. 2014. “I Remember it like it was Interesting”: Zur Theorie von Salienz und Pertinenz. Linguistik Online 66(4/14): 31–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rácz, P. 2012. Operationalising Salience: Definite Article Reduction in the North of England. English Language and Linguistics 16(1): 57–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Regier, T. and Zheng, M. 2007. Attention to Endpoints: A Cross-Linguistic Constraint on Spatial Meaning. Cognitive Science 31(4): 705–719. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmiedtová, B., von Stutterheim, C. and Carroll, M. 2011. Language-Specific Patterns in Event Construal of Advanced Second Language Speakers. In Thinking and Speaking in Two Languages, A. Pavlenko (ed.), 66–107. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skordos, D., Bunger, A., Richards, C., Selimis, S., Trueswell, J. and Papafragou, A. 2019. Motion Verbs and Memory for Motion Events. Cognitive Neuropsychology 37(5–6): 254–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2003. Language and Thought Online: Cognitive Consequences of Linguistic Relativity. In Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, D. Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow (eds), 157–192. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. and Rohde, A. 2004. The Goal Bias in the Encoding of Motion Events. In Studies in Linguistic Motivation, G. Radden and K.-U. Panther (eds), 249–268. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (ed.). 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Von Stutterheim, C. 2003. Linguistic Structure and Information Organisation: The Case of very Advanced Learners. EuroSLA Yearbook 3(1): 183–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Von Stutterheim, C., Andermann, M., Carroll, M., Flecken, M. and Schmiedtová, B. 2012. How Grammaticized Concepts Shape Event Conceptualization in Language Production: Insights from Linguistic Analysis, Eye Tracking Data, and Memory Performance. Linguistics 50(4): 833–867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Von Stutterheim, C., Carroll, M. and Klein, W. 2009. New Perspectives in Analyzing Aspectual Distinctions across Languages. In The Expression of Time, W. Klein and P. Li (eds), 195–216. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Von Stutterheim, C. and Lambert, M. 2005. Cross-Linguistic Analysis of Temporal Perspectives in Text Production. In The Structure of Learner Varieties, H. Hendriks (ed.), 203–230. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Von Stutterheim, C. and Nüse, R. 2003. Processes of Conceptualization in Language Production: Language-Specific Perspectives and Event Construal. Linguistics 41(5): 851–881. DOI logoGoogle Scholar