Article published in:
Metaphor and Communication
Edited by Elisabetta Gola and Francesca Ervas
[Metaphor in Language, Cognition, and Communication 5] 2016
► pp. 4760
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

No author info given
2019.  In Sensory Linguistics [Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, 20], Crossref logo
No author info given
2019.  In Sensory Linguistics [Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, 20],  pp. 235 ff. Crossref logo
Kumcu, Alper
2021. Linguistic Synesthesia in Turkish: A Corpus-based Study of Crossmodal Directionality. Metaphor and Symbol 36:4  pp. 241 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 october 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Asher, J.E., Lamb, J.A., Brocklebank, D., Cazier, J.B., Maestrini, E., Addis, L., Sen, M., Baron-Cohen, S., & Monaco, A.P.
(2009) A Whole-Genome Scan and Fine-Mapping Linkage Study of Auditory-Visual Synesthesia Reveals Evidence of Linkage to Chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 279–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baroni, M., Bernardini, S., Ferraresi, A., & Zanchetta, E.
(2009) The WaCky Wide Web: A collection of very large linguistically processed Web-Crawled corpora. Language Resources and Evaluation, 43, 209–226. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L.W.
(2008) Grounding cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bazzanella, C.
(1999) La metafora tra mente e discorso. Lingua e Stile, 34, 150–158.Google Scholar
Bazzanella C.
(Ed.) (2009) La forza cognitiva della metafora. Paradigmi, 27(1).Google Scholar
Bonnans, S.
., & Noble, A.C 1(993) Effect of sweetener type and of sweetener and acid level on temporal perception of sweetness, sourness and fruitiness. Chemical Senses, 18, 273–283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cacciari, C.
(1999) La metafora fra linguaggio ed esperienza percettiva. Lingua e stile, 34(2), 159–166.Google Scholar
(2005) I sensi della metafora: sul rapporto fra percezione e linguaggio. In A.M. Lorusso (Ed.), Metafora e conoscenza. Da Aristotele al cognitivismo contemporaneo (pp. 321–348). Milano: Bompiani.Google Scholar
Casadei, F.
(2003) Per un bilancio della semantica cognitiva. In L. Gaeta & S. Luraghi (Eds.), Introduzione alla linguistica cognitiva (pp. 37–55). Roma: Carocci.Google Scholar
Cytowic, R.E.
(2002) Synesthesia: A union of the senses. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cytowic, R.E., & Eagleman, D.M.
(2009) Wednesday is indigo blue: Discovering the brain of synesthesia. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Day, S.
(1996) Synaesthesia and synaesthetic metaphors. Psyche, 2(32), http://​home​.comcast​.net​/~sean​.day​/html​/bibliography​.html.Google Scholar
De Araujo, I.E., & Rolls, E.T.
(2004) Representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(12), 3086–3093. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elder, R.S., & Krishna, A.
(2010) The effect of advertising copy on sensory thoughts and perceived taste. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 748–756. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, V., Bergen, B.K., & Zinken, J.
(Eds.) (2007) The cognitive linguistic reader. London: Equinox Publisher.Google Scholar
Gagliasso, E., & Frezza, G.
(2010) Metafore del vivente. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Galton, F.
(1880) Visualised numerals. Nature, 22, 494–495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.W.
(2005) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guest, S., & Spence, C.
(2002) What role does multisensory integration play in the visuotactile perception of texture? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 63–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hampe, B., & Grady J.E.
(Eds.) (2005) From perception to meaning: image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin-New York: Mouton De Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hancock, P.J.B.
(2006) Monozygotic twins’ colour-number association, a case study. Cortex, 42(2), 147–150. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holz, P.
(2007) Cognition, olfaction and linguistic creativity. In M. Plümacher & P. Holz (Eds.), Speaking of colors and odors (pp. 185–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Indurkhya, B.
(1992) Metaphor and cognition: An interactionist approach. Dordrecth: Kluwer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.
(1987) The body in the mind. Chicago: Longman.Google Scholar
Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., & Jessel, T.M.
(2000) Principles of neural science. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Krishna, A., & Maureen, M.
(2008) Does touch affect taste? The perceptual transfer of product container haptic cues. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 807–818. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Monopoli, D., & Cacciari, C.
(2009) Olfatto: un senso ‘senza parole’? Paradigmi, 27(1), 147–162.Google Scholar
Popova, Y.
(2005) Image schemas and verbal synaesthesia. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From perception to meaning: image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 395–419). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V.S., & Hubbard, E.M.
(2003) The phenomenology of synaesthesia. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(8), 49–57.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V.S., Hubbard, E.M., & Butcher, P.A.
(2004) Synethesia, cross-Activation, and the foundations of neuroepistemology. In G. Calvert, C. Spence, & B.E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processes (pp. 867–884). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ronga, I., Bazzanella, C., Rossi, F., & Iannetti G.D.
(2012) Linguistic synaesthesia, perceptual synaesthesia and the interaction between multiple sensory modalities. Pragmatics and Cognition, 20(1), 135–167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ronga, I.
(2014) La multimodalità del sistema nervoso. APhEx, 10, 182–222.Google Scholar
Schifferstein, H.N.J., & Verlegh, P.W.J.
(1996) The role of congruency and pleasantness in odor-induced taste enhancement. Acta Psychologica, 94, 87–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shen, Y., & Cohen, M.
(1998) How come silence is sweet but sweetness is not silent: a cognitive account of directionality in poetic synaesthesia. Language and Literature, 7, 123–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shen, Y., & Gil, D.
(2008) Sweet fragrances from Indonesia: A universal principle governing directionality in synaesthetic metaphors. In W. van Peer & J. Auracher (Eds.), New beginning or the study of literature (pp. 49–72). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Simner, J.
(2011) Defining synaesthesia. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stampanoni, C.R.
(1995) Influence of acid and sugar content on sweetness, sourness and the flavour profile of beverages and sherbets. Food Quality and Preference, 4, 169–176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J.R.
(1995) Linguistic categorization. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ullmann, S.
(1957) The principles of semantics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Verhagen, E.
(2006), The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: Sensory integration. Neuroscience and biobehavioural review, 30, 613–650. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.M.
(1976) Synaesthetic adjectives: A possible law of semantic change. Language, 52, 461–478. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yu, N.
(2003) Synaesthetic metaphor: A cognitive perspective. Journal of Literary Semantics, 32, 19–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar