Article published in:
Semantics and Psychology of Complex Words
Edited by Christina L. Gagné and Thomas L. Spalding
[The Mental Lexicon 15:1] 2020
► pp. 6278
References

References

Amenta, S., & Crepaldi, D.
(2012) Morphological processing as we know it: An analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Amenta, S., Crepaldi, D., & Marelli, M.
(2020) Consistency measures individuate dissociating semantic modulations in priming paradigms: A new look on semantics in the processing of (complex) words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Amenta, S., Marelli, M., & Crepaldi, D.
(2015) The fruitless effort of growing a fruitless tree: Early morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic effects in sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(5), 1587.Google Scholar
Amenta, S., Marelli, M., & Sulpizio, S.
(2017) From sound to meaning: Phonology-to-Semantics mapping in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin &Rreview, 24(3), 887–893. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S., & Lo, S.
(2013) Is morphological priming stronger for transparent than opaque words? It depends on individual differences in spelling and vocabulary. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 279–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Chuang, Y. -Y., Shafaei-Bajestan, E., & Blevins, J. P.
(2019) The discriminative lexicon: A unified computational model for the lexicon and lexical processing in comprehension and production grounded not in (de)composition but in linear discriminative learning. Complexity, 4895891. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Milin, P., Đurđević, D. F., Hendrix, P., & Marelli, M.
(2011) An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review, 118(3), 438. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B. K.
(2004) The psychological reality of phonaesthemes. Language, 80(2), 290–311. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, J. P.
(2016) Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, L.
(1933) Language. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Bojanowski, P., Grave, E., Joulin, A., & Mikolov, T.
(2017) Enriching word vectors with subword information. Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 5, 135–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A.
(2005) Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a “hat” in “that”? Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 131–143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, J. M., Gagné, C. L., Spalding, T. L., & Lõo, K.
(2020) Detecting spelling errors in compound and pseudocompound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(3), 580–602.Google Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Marelli, M., & Amenta, S.
(2019) For a probabilistic and multidisciplinary approach to the investigation of morphological processing. Cortex, 116, 1–3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Jong IV, N. H., Schreuder, R., & Harald Baayen, R.
(2000) The morphological family size effect and morphology. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4–5), 329–365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D., & Grainger, J.
(2005) Masked cross-modal morphological priming: Unravelling morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic influences in early word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(1–2), 75–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Kinoshita, S., Carreiras, M., & Norris, D.
(2011) Is morpho-orthographic decomposition purely orthographic? Evidence from masked priming in the same-different task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4–6), 509–529. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
El-Bialy, R., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2013) Processing of English compounds is sensitive to the constituents – semantic transparency. The Mental Lexicon, 8, 75–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B.
(2000) Are morphological effects distinguishable from the effects of shared meaning and shared form? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1431.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., O’Connor, P. A., & del Prado Martín, F. M.
(2009) Early morphological processing is morphosemantic and not simply morpho-orthographic: A violation of form-then-meaning accounts of word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 684–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., Deutsch, A., & Forster, K. I.
(2000) Decomposing morphologically complex words in a nonlinear morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(3), 751–765.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L.
(2001) Relation and lexical priming during the interpretation of noun-noun combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 236–254.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Shoben, E. J.
(1997) Influence of thematic relations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 71–87.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2009) Constituent integration during the processing of compound words: Does it involve the use of relational structures? Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 20–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., & Marelli, M.
(2019) Enter sandman: Compound processing and semantic transparency in a compositional perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 1872–1882.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2006) Conceptual combination: Implications for the mental lexicon. In Libben, G., & Jarema, G. (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 145–168). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Günther, F., & Marelli, M.
(2020) Trying to make it work: Semantic effects in the processing of compound “nonwords”. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Advance online publication. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., Marelli, M., & Bölte, J.
(2020) Semantic transparency effects in German compounds: A large dataset and multiple-task investigation. Behavior Research Methods. Advance online publication. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., Petilli, M. A., & Marelli, M.
(2020) Semantic transparency is not invisibility: A computational model of perceptually-grounded conceptual combination in word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 112, 104104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., Rinaldi, L., & Marelli, M.
(2019) Vector-space models of semantic representation from a cognitive perspective: A discussion of common misconceptions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 1006–1033. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Günther, F., Smolka, E., & Marelli, M.
(2019) ‘Understanding – differs between English and German: Capturing systematic language differences of complex words. Cortex, 116, 168–175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(2004) Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111(3), 662. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Z.
(1954) Distributional Structure. Word, 10, 146–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hendrix, P., & Sun, C. C.
(2020) A word or two about nonwords: Frequency, semantic neighborhood density, and orthography-to-semantics consistency effects for nonwords in the lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hyönä, J., & Pollatsek, A.
(1998) Reading Finnish compound words: Eye fixations are affected by component morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1612.Google Scholar
Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2011) Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 406–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. N., Willits, J., & Dennis, S.
(2015) Models of semantic memory. In J. Busemeyer, Z. Wang, J. Townsend, & A. Eidels (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Mathematical and Computational Psychology (pp. 232–254). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Juhasz, B. J.
(2007) The influence of semantic transparency on eye movements during English compound word recognition. In Eye Movements (pp. 373–389). Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T.
(1997) A solution to Plato’s problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Libben, G.
(2014) The nature of compounds: A psychocentric perspective. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31, 8–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017) The quantum metaphor and the organization of words in the mind. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 1(1), 49–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Libben, G., Gibson, M., Yoon, Y. B., & Sandra, D.
(2003) Compound fracture: The role of semantic transparency and morphological headedness. Brain and Language, 84, 50–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Longtin, C. M., Segui, J., & Hallé, P. A.
(2003) Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(3), 313–334. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lund, K., & Burgess, C.
(1996) Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(2), 203–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., & Amenta, S.
(2018) A database of orthography-semantics consistency (OSC) estimates for 15,017 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 1482–1495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., Amenta, S., & Crepaldi, D.
(2015) Semantic transparency in free stems: The effect of Orthography-Semantics Consistency on word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(8), 1571–1583. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., & Baroni, M.
(2015) Affixation in semantic space: Modeling morpheme meanings with compositional distributional semantics. Psychological Review, 122, 485–515. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., & Luzzatti, C.
(2012) Frequency effects in the processing of Italian nominal compounds: Modulation of headedness and semantic transparency. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 644–664. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., Amenta, S., Morone, E. A., & Crepaldi, D.
(2013) Meaning is in the beholder’s eye: Morpho-semantic effects in masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 534–541. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marelli, M., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2017) Compounding as Abstract Operation in Semantic Space: A data-driven, large-scale model for relational effects in the processing of novel compounds. Cognition, 166, 207–224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older, L.
(1994) Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101(1), 3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mikolov, T., Chen, K., Corrado, G., & Dean, J.
(2013) Efficient estimation of word representations in vector space. arXiv:1301.3781v3.Google Scholar
Milin, P., Feldman, L. B., Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., & Baayen, H.
(2017) Discrimination in lexical decision. PLoS ONE, 12(2), Article e0171935. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Milin, P., Đurđević, D. F., & del Prado Martín, F. M.
(2009) The simultaneous effects of inflectional paradigms and classes on lexical recognition: Evidence from Serbian. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(1), 50–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, J., & Lapata, M.
(2010) Composition in distributional models of semantics. Cognitive Science, 34, 1388–1429. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Plaut, D. C., & Gonnerman, L. M.
(2000) Are non-semantic morphological effects incompatible with a distributed connectionist approach to lexical processing? Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4–5), 445–485. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H.
(2008) Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 942–971. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(7–8), 942–971. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B.
(2004) The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1090–1098. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K.
(2000) Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A timecourse study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 507–537. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Recchia, G., & Jones, M. N.
(2009) More data trumps smarter algorithms: Comparing pointwise mutual information with latent semantic analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3), 647–656. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R.
(1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory, 2, 64–99.Google Scholar
Sahel, S., Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., & Weingarten, R.
(2008) Written production of German compounds: Effects of lexical frequency and semantic transparency. Written Language & Literacy, 11, 211–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schäfer, M.
(2018) The semantic transparency of English compound nouns. Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Schmidtke, D., Van Dyke, J. A., & Kuperman, V.
(2018) Individual variability in the semantic processing of English compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 421–439.Google Scholar
Schmidtke, D., Gagné, C. L., Kuperman, V., Spalding, T. L., & Tucker, B. V.
(2018) Conceptual relations compete during auditory and visual compound word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33, 923–942. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Preller, K. H., & Eulitz, C.
(2014) ‘Verstehen’(‘understand’) primes ‘stehen’(‘stand’): Morphological structure overrides semantic compositionality in the lexical representation of German complex verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 16–36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Westbury, C., & Hollis, G.
(2019) Conceptualizing syntactic categories as semantic categories: Unifying part-of-speech identification and semantics using co-occurrence vector averaging. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 1371–1398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wurm, L. H.
(2000) Auditory processing of polymorphemic pseudowords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 255–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar