Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 10:3 (2015) ► pp.435457
References
Audacity Team
(2013) Audacity®(Version 2.0.5). [Computer program]. Retrieved February 10 2014, from [URL]
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S
(2015) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–9, [URL].Google Scholar
Bertram, R., Kuperman, V., Baayen, R.H., & Hyönä, J
(2011) The hyphen as a segmentation cue in triconstituent compound processing: It’s getting better all the time. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 52(6), 530–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Vitu, F., & Schroyens, W
(1996) The right visual field advantage and the Optimal Viewing Position Effect: On the relation between foveal and parafoveal word recognition. Neuropsychology, 10(3), 385–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Nazir, T
(2005) Visual constraints in written word recognition: Evidence from the optimal viewing-position effect. Journal of Research in Reading, 281, 216–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J.D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J
(1993) PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25(2), 257–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, L., & Mehler, J
(1996) Click monitoring revisited: An on-line study of sentence comprehension. Memory & cognition, 24(1), 94–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Norris, D
(1979) Monitoring sentence comprehension. In W.E. Cooper & E.C.T. Walker (Eds.), Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett (pp. 113–134). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Faust, M., Kravetz, S., & Babkoff, H
(1993) Hemispheric specialization or reading habits: Evidence from lexical decision research with Hebrew words and sentences. Brain and Language, 441, 254–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.A., & Bever, T.G
(1965) The psychological reality of linguistic segments. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 41, 414–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, D.P., & Zatorre, R.J
(1981) A right-ear advantage for dichotic listening in bilingual children. Brain and Language, 13(2), 389–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holmes, V.M., & Forster, K.I
(1972) Click location and syntactic structure. Perception & Psychophysics, 12(1), 9–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hugdahl, K
(2011) Fifty years of dichotic listening research – Still going and going and… Brain and Cognition, 761, 211–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) Lateralization of cognitive processing in the brain. Acta Psychologica, 1051, 211–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ip, K., & Hoosain, R
(1993) Dichotic listening of Chinese and English words. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 36(3), 140–143.Google Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M
(2010) Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 627–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimura, D
(2011) From ear to brain. Brain and Cognition, 761, 214–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1967) Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 31, 163–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1966) Dual functional asymmetry of the brain in visual perception. Neuropsychologia, 41, 275–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1961a) Some effects of temporal-lobe damage on auditory perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 151, 156–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1961b) Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 151, 166–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, P., & Broadbent, D.E
(1960) Perception of sequence in auditory events. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 162–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laine, M., & Koivisto, M
(1998) Lexical access to inflected words as measured by lateralized visual lexical decision. Psychological Research, 611, 220–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leff, A
(2004) A historical review of the representation of the visual field in primary visual cortex with special reference to the neural mechanisms underlying macular sparing. Brain and Language, 881, 268–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Libben, G
(2003) Morphological Parsing and Morphological Structure. In E.M.H. Assink & D. Sandra (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross language studies (pp. 221–239). New York: Kluwer Academic. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moen, I
(1993) Functional lateralization of the perception of Norwegian Word Tones – Evidence from a dichotic listening Experiment. Brain and Language, 44(4), 400–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyers, J.E., Roberts, R.J., Bayless, J.D., Volkert, K., & Evitts, P.E
(2002) Dichotic listening: expanded norms and clinical application. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17(1), 79–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, J.K., & Jacobs, A.M
(1992) Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 181, 185–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL]Google Scholar
Reber, A.S
(1973) Locating clicks in sentences: Left, center, and right. Perception and Psychophysics, 13(1), 133–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saetrevik, B., & Hugdahl, K
(2007) Priming inhibits the right ear advantage in dichotic listening: Implications for auditory laterality. Neuropsychologia, 451, 282–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M
(1967) Identification of consonants and vowels presented to left and right ears. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 191, 59–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Shankweiler, D
(1970) Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 481, 579–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., & Ransijn, J
(2013) LMERConvenienceFunctions: A suite of functions to back-fit fixed effects and forward-fit random effects, as well as other miscellaneous functions. R package version 21.5. [URL]Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J.A
(2001) Dichotic perception of Mandarin tones by Chinese and American listeners. Brain and Language, 78(3), 332–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westerhausen, R., Moosmann, M., Alho, K., Medvedev, S., Hamalainen, H., & Hugdahl, K
(2009) Top-down and bottom-up interaction: Manipulating the dichotic listening ear advantages. Brain Research, 12501, 183–189 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, B.E., & Halwes, T
(1983) Increasing the power of dichotic methods: The fused rhymed words test. Neuropsychologia, 211, 59–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young, A.W., & Ellis, A.W
(1985) Different methods of lexical access for words presented in the left and right visual hemifields. Brain and Language, 241, 326–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar