Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 11:1 (2016) ► pp.2654
References (52)
Anderson, S. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Basnight-Brown, D.M., Chen, L., Hua, S., Kostić, A., & Feldman, L.B. (2007). Monolingual and bilingual recognition of regular and irregular English verbs: Sensitivity to form similarity varies with first language experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 571, 65–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blevins, J.P. (2000). Markedness and blocking in German declensional paradigms. In B. Stiebels & D. Wunderlich (Eds.), Lexicon in focus (pp. 83–103). Berlin: Akademie Verlag.Google Scholar
Bosch, S., Krause, H., & Leminen, A. (2016). The time-course of morphosyntactic and semantic priming in late bilinguals: A study of German adjectives. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (2005). Discontinuous morphology in time: Incremental masked Arabic priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 201, 207–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstairs, A. (1987). Allomorphy in inflexion. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 161–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Balkhair, L., Schutter, J.-S., & Cunnings, I. (2013). The time course of morphological processing in a second language. Second Language Research, 291, 7–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Eisenbeiss, S., Hadler, M., & Sonnenstuhl, I. (2001). The mental representation of inflected words: An experimental study of adjectives and verbs in German. Language, 771, 510–543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Neubauer, K. (2010). Morphology, frequency, and the processing of derived words in native and non-native speakers. Lingua 1201, 2627–2637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C.E., & Tremblay, A. (2015). Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 524–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Colheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2010). ‘Fell’ primes ‘fall’, but does ‘bell’ prime ‘ball’? Masked priming with irregularly-inflected primes. Journal of Memory and Language, 631, 83–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2005). What makes learning a second-language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Language Learning, 551, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimroth, C. (2008). Age effects on the process of L2 acquisition? Evidence from the acquisition of negation and finiteness in L2 German. Language Learning, 581, 117–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L.B., Kostic, A., Basnight-Brown, D.M., Filipovic Djurdjevic, D., & Pastizzo, M.J. (2010). Morphological facilitation for regular and irregular verb formations in native and non-native speakers: Little evidence for two distinct mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Azuma, T. (2000). Masked priming for prefixed words with bound stems: Does submit prime permit? Language and Cognitive Processes, 151, 539–561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 101, 680–698. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Forster, J.C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 351, 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonnerman, L., Seidenberg, M., & Andersen, E. (2007). Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1361, 323–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyer, V., & Clahsen, H. (2015). Late bilinguals see a scan in scanner AND in scandal: Dissecting formal overlap from morphological priming in the processing of derived words. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 543–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C.N. (2008). Proficiency level and the interaction of lexical and morphosyntactic information during L2 sentence processing. Language Learning, 581, 875–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C.N., & Bobb, S.C. (2009). The processing and comprehension of wh-questions among second language speakers of German. Applied Psycholinguistics, 301, 603–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C.N., & Dussias, P.E. (2009). Cross-linguistic differences and their impact on L2 sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 121, 65–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., Fleischhauer, E., & Clahsen, H. (2013). Stem allomorphy and affixation in morphological processing: A cross-modal priming study with late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 161, 924–933. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 603–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 571, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keating, G. (2009). Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 591, 503–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirkici, B., & Clahsen, H. (2013). Inflection and derivation in native and non-native language processing: Masked priming experiments on Turkish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 161, 776–794. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leminen, A., & Clahsen, H. (2014). Brain potentials to inflected adjectives: Beyond storage and decomposition. Brain Research, 15431, 223–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 181, 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longtin, C.-M., Segui, J., & Hallé, P.A. (2003). Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 181, 313–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longtin, C.-M., & Meunier, F. (2005). Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 531, 26–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. (2007). Morphological processes in language comprehension. In G. Gaskel (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 175–193). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Bozic, M., & Randall, B. (2008). Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form and meaning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 231, 394–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. (2011). First and second language acquisition: Parallels and differences. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, K., & Clahsen, H. (2009). Decomposition of inflected words in a second language: An experimental study of German participles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 311, 403–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Opitz, A., Regel, S., Müller, G., & Friederici, A.D. (2013). Neurophysiological evidence for morphological underspecification in German strong adjective inflection. Language, 891, 231–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parodi, T. (2000). Finiteness and verb placement in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 161, 355–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Penke, M., Janssen, U., & Eisenbeiss, S. (2004). Psycholinguistic evidence for the underspecification of morphosyntactic features. Brain and Language, 901, 423–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., & Davis, M.H. (2003). Reading morphologically-complex words: Some thoughts from masked priming. In S. Kinoshita & S.J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the art (pp. 279–305). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M.H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 111, 1090–1098. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M.H., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., & Tyler, L.K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 151, 507–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, M., & Felser, C. (2010). Sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations in English as a second language. Second Language, 91, 101–118.Google Scholar
Schriefers, H., Friederici, A.D., & Graetz, P. (1992). Inflectional and derivational morphology in the mental lexicon: Symmetries and asymmetries in repetition priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 441, 373–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva, R., & Clahsen, H. (2008). Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 111, 245–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stump, G. (2001). Inflectional morphology: A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanner, D., Osterhout, L., & Herschensohn, J. (2009). Snapshots of grammaticalization: Differential electrophysiological responses to grammatical anomalies with increasing L2 exposure. In J. Chandlee, M. Franchini, S. Lord, & G.-M. Rheiner (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 528–539). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Voga, M., Anastassiadis-Symeonidis, A., & Giraudo, H. (2014). Does morphology play a role in L2 processing? Two masked priming experiments with Greek speakers of ESL. Linguisticae Investigationes, 371, 338–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, D. (1997). Der unterspezifizierte Artikel. In C. Dürscheid, K.-H. Ramers, & M. Schwarz (Eds.), Sprache im Fokus (pp. 47–55). Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Zwicky, A.M. (1985). How to describe inflection. Berkeley Linguistics Society, 111, 372–386.Google Scholar
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Opitz, Andreas, Denisa Bordag & Alberto Furgoni
2022. Mental Representation of Word Family Structure: The Case of German Infinitives, Conversion Nouns and Other Morphologically Related Forms. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Opitz, Andreas, Denisa Bordag & Alberto Furgoni
2024. Morphosyntactic underspecification affects the processing of verbal forms at different levels of abstraction in L1 and L2 German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Kimppa, Lilli, Yury Shtyrov, Suzanne C.A. Hut, Laura Hedlund, Miika Leminen & Alina Leminen
2019. Acquisition of L2 morphology by adult language learners. Cortex 116  pp. 74 ff. DOI logo
BOSCH, SINA & ALINA LEMINEN
2018. ERP priming studies of bilingual language processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21:3  pp. 462 ff. DOI logo
Giraudo, Hélène & Serena Dal Maso
2018. Towards a Constructional Approach of L2 Morphological Processing. In The Construction of Words [Studies in Morphology, 4],  pp. 603 ff. DOI logo
Reifegerste, Jana & Harald Clahsen

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.