Functionalism in the lexicon
Where is it, and how did it get there?
Why do languages have the words they have, and not some other set of words? While certainly there is some arbitrariness in the lexicon (English ‘frog’ vs. Spanish ‘rana’), there is just as surely some systematicity or functionality in it as well. What exactly might the nature of this systematicity or functionality be? For example, might the lexicon be efficiently adapted for communication, learning, memory storage, retrieval, or other cognitive functions? This paper critically reviews evidence that natural language lexicons efficiently carve up semantic fields (e.g., color, space, kinship) and have phonological forms that are similarly efficient when the aggregate lexicon is considered. The paper also suggests additional ways functionalism in lexicons might be assessed, and speculates on how functional lexicons may have arisen.
References (106)
Atran, S. (1995). Classifying nature across cultures. In E.E. Smith & D.N. Osherson (Eds.), Thinking: An invitation to cognitive science (vol. 31, pp. 131–174). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baronchelli, A., Gong, T., Puglisi, A., & Loreto, V. (2010). Modeling the emergence of universality in color naming patterns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(6), 2403–2407.
Battaglia, P.W., Hamrick, J.B., & Tenenbaum, J.B. (2013). Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(45), 18327–18332.
Beach, L.R. (1964). Cue probabilism and inference behavior. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 78(5–6), 1.
Berlin B., Kay P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Blei, D.M. (2012). Probabilistic topic models. Communications of the ACM, 55(4), 77.
Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J., Golinkoff, R.M., & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16(4), 298–304.
Boster, J. (2005). Emotion categories across languages. In H. Cohen & C. LeFebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (pp. 187–222). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bowerman, M., & Pederson, E. (1992). Cross-linguistic studies of spatial semantic organization. In Annual report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics (pp. 53–56).
Brashears, M.E. (2013). Humans use compression heuristics to improve the recall of social networks. Scientific Reports, 3, 1513.
Brentari, D., & Coppola, M. (2013). What sign language creation teaches us about language. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(2), 201–211.
Bybee, J. (1999). Usage-based phonology. In M. Darnell, E. Moravcsik, F. Newmeyer, M. Noonan, & K. Wheatley (Eds.), Functionalism and formalism in linguistics, Volume I: General papers (pp. 211–242). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language, 82(4), 711–733.
Caramazza, A., & Mahon, B.Z. (2003). The organization of conceptual knowledge: the evidence from category-specific semantic deficits. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(8), 354–361.
Carstensen, A., Xu, J., Smith, C.T., & Regier, T. (2015). Language evolution in the lab tends toward informative communication. In D.C. Noelle, R. Dale, A.S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C.D. Jennings, & P.P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Casasanto, D., & Lupyan, G. (2015). All concepts are ad hoc concepts. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: New directions (pp. 543–566). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Corter, J.E., & Gluck, M.A. (1992). Explaining basic categories: Feature predictability and information. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 291–303.
Croft, W. (1995). Autonomy and functionalist linguistics. Language, 71(3), 490–532.
Croft, W. (2003). Typology and universals (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cutler, A., Hawkins, J.A., & Gilligan, G. (1986). The suffixing preference: A processing explanation. Linguistics, 231. 723–758.
de Boer, B. (2000). Self organization in vowel systems. Journal of Phonetics, 281, 441–465.
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D.E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M.H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615.
Ferrer i Cancho, R., & Sole, R.V. (2003). Least effort and the origins of scaling in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(3), 788–791.
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., & Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. (2011). Information content versus word length in random typing. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment, 2011(12), L12002.
Fowler, C.A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers’ signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(5), 489–504.
Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., & Roberts, G. (2012). Experimental semiotics. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(8), 477–493.
Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. MIT press.
Garner, W.R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.
Giavazzi, M. (2010). The phonetics of metrical prominence and its consequences for segmental phonology. PhD thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Graff, P. (2012). Communicative efficiency in the lexicon. PhD Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Griffiths, T.L., Steyvers, M., & Firl, A. (2007). Google and the mind: Predicting fluency with Pagerank. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1069–1076.
Griffiths, T.L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J.B.T. (2007). Topics in semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114(2), 211–244.
Griffiths, T.L., & Tenenbaum, J.B. (2006). Optimal predictions in everyday cognition. Psychological Science, 171, 767–773.
Haspelmath, M. (1999). Optimality and diachronic adaptation. Zeitschrift Für Sprachwissenschaft, 18(2), 180–205.
Haspelmath, M. (2003). The geometry of grammatical meaning: Semantic maps and cross-linguistic comparison. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The new psychology of language (vol. 21, pp. 211–242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hills, T., Maouene, M., & Maouene, J. Sheya, A., & Smith, L. (2009a). Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks preferential attachment or preferential acquisition? Psychological Science, 20(6), 729–739.
Hills, T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., & Smith, L. (2009b). Categorical structure among shared features in networks of early-learned nouns. Cognition, 1121, 381–396.
Hurford, J.R. (2003). Why synonymy is rare: Fitness is in the speaker. In W. Banzhaf, T. Christaller, P. Dittrich, J.T. Kim, & J. Ziegler (Eds.), Advances in artificial life– Proceedings of the 7th European Conference on Artificial Life (ECAL), lecture notes in artificial intelligence (vol. 28011, pp. 442). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Jaeger, T.F., & Tily, H. (2011). On language “utility”: processing complexity and communicative efficiency. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 323–335.
Kemp, C., & Regier, T. (2012). Kinship categories across languages reflect general communicative principles. Science, 336(6084), 1049–1054.
Khetarpal, N., Majid, A., & Regier, T. (2009). Spatial terms reflect near-optimal spatial categories. In N. Taatgen, et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.
Khetarpal, N., Neveu, G., Majid, A., Michael, L., & Regier, T. (2013). Spatial terms across languages support near-optimal communication: Evidence from Peruvian Amazonia, and computational analyses. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.
King, R. (1967). Functional load and sound change. Language 431, 831–852.
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: an experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(31), 10681–10686.
Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T.L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(12), 5241–5245.
Kleinberg, J.M. (2000). Navigation in a small world. Nature, 406(6798), 845.
Kurumada, C., Meylan, S.C., & Frank, M.C. (2013). Zipfian frequency distributions facilitate word segmentation in context. Cognition, 1271, 439–453.
Landauer, T.K., Foltz, P.W., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25(2–3), 259–284.
Lelu, A. (2014). Jean-Baptiste Estoup and the origins of Zipf s law: A stenographer with a scientific mind (1868–1950). BEIO, Boletín de Estadística e Investigación Operativa, 30(1), 66–77.
Lindblom, B. (1986). Phonetic universals in vowel systems. In John Ohala & Jeri Jaeger (Eds.), Experimental phonology (pp. 13–44). Orlando: Academic Press.
Loreto, V., Mukherjee, A., & Tria, F. (2012). On the origin of the hierarchy of color names. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(18), 6819–6824.
Mahowald, K., Fedorenko, E., Piantadosi, S.T., & Gibson, E. (2013). Info/information theory: speakers choose shorter words in predictive contexts. Cognition, 126(2), 313–318.
Majid, A., & Burenhult, N. (2014). Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition, 130(2), 266–270.
Majid, A., Enfield, N.J., & van Staden, M. (2006). Parts of the body: Cross-linguistic categorisation (Special Issue). Language Sciences, 281(2–3).
Malt, B.C., Sloman, S.A., Gennari, S., Shi, M., & Wang, Y. (1999). Knowing versus naming: Similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifacts. Journal of Memory and Language, 401, 230–262.
Marcus, G.F., & Davis, E. (2013). How robust are probabilistic models of higher-level cognition? Psychological Science, 24(12), 2351–2360.
Margolis, E., & Laurence, S. (2014). Concepts. In E.N. Zalta (Ed.), The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2014 Edition). Retrieved from <[URL]>.
Markman, E.M., & Wachtel, G.F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive psychology, 20(2), 121–157.
Martin, A. 2007. The evolving Lexicon. PhD Thesis, University of California, Los Angeles.
Martinet, A. (1952). Function, structure, and sound change. Word, 81, 1–32.
Miller, G.A. (1957). Some effects of intermittent silence. The American Journal of Psychology, 70(2), 311–314.
Moscoso del Prado Martin, F. (2013). The missing baselines in arguments for the optimal efficiency of languages. Csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu, 1032–1037. Retrieved from [URL]
Murphy, G. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
Newman, M.E. (2002). Assortative mixing in networks. Physical review letters, 89(20), 208701.
Piantadosi, S.T. (2014). Zipf’s word frequency law in natural language: A critical review and future directions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–35.
Piantadosi, S.T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2011). Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(9), 3526–3529.
Piantadosi, S.T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2012). The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition, 122(3), 280–291.
Piantadosi, S.T., Tily, H.J., & Gibson, E. (2009). The communicative lexicon hypothesis. In Proceedings of the
31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society
(pp. 2582–2587).
Piantadosi, S., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2013). Information content versus word length in natural language: A reply to Ferrer-i-Cancho and Moscoso del Prado Martin (2011). arXiv Preprint arXiv:1307.6726, 2011, 1–8. Retrieved from [URL]
Posner, M.I., & Keele, S.W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 771, 353–363.
Posner, M.I., & Keele, S.W. (1970). Retention of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 831, 304–308.
Reed, K. (1972). Pattern recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 4071, 382–407.
Regier, T., Kay, P., & Khetarpal, N. (2007). Color naming reflects optimal partitions of color space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(4), 1436–1441.
Regier, T., Kemp, C., & Kay, P. (2015). Word meanings across languages support efficient communication. In B. MacWhinney & W.O. Grady (Eds.), The handbook of language emergence (pp. 237–263). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Richie, R., Kaufmann, S., & Tabor, W. (2014). An LSA-based method for estimating word meaning specificity: An application to an account of Zipf’s Law. Poster presented at the
9th annual Mental Lexicon Conference
, Niagara-on-the-Lake, Ontario, Canada.
Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1291, 369–398.
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I.R.L., & Shapiro, L.R. (2005). Color categories: Evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 501, 378–411.
Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T.E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.
Rosch, E., Simpson, C., & Miller, R.S. (1976). Structural bases of typicality effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 491–502.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B.B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Reprinted in: Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (Eds.) (1999). Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sagi, E., Kaufmann, S., & Clark, B. (2011). Tracing semantic change with latent semantic analysis. In K. Allan & J.A. Robinson (Eds.), Current methods in historical semantics (pp. 161–183). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
Scott-Phillips, T.C., & Kirby, S. (2010). Language evolution in the laboratory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(9), 411–417.
Scott-Phillips, T.C., Dickins, T.E., & West, S.A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate–proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 38–47.
Silvey, C., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2013). Communication leads to the emergence of sub-optimal category structures. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1312–1317). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Steels, L., & Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: A case study for colour. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 469–489; discussion 489–529.
Steels, L. (2011). Modeling the cultural evolution of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(4), 339–356.
Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J.B. (2005). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29(1), 41–78.
Thompson, B., Silvey, C., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2014). The effect of communication on category structure. In E.A. Cartmill, S. Roberts, H. Lyn, & H. Cornish (Eds.), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 10th International Conference (EVOLANG10). Singapore: World Scientific.
Thompson, G.W., & Kello, C.T. (2014). Walking across Wikipedia: A scale-free network model of semantic memory retrieval. Frontiers in Psychology, 51, 86.
Thompson, D. (1917). On growth and form. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Vitevitch, M.S., Chan, K.Y., & Goldstein, R. (2014). Insights into failed lexical retrieval from network science. Cognitive Psychology, 681, 1–32.
Wedel, A., Jackson, S., & Kaplan, A. (2013). Functional load and the lexicon: Evidence that syntactic category and frequency relationships in minimal lemma pairs predict the loss of phoneme contrasts in language change. Language and Speech, 56(3), 395–417.
Wedel, A., Kaplan, A., & Jackson, S. (2013). High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: a corpus study. Cognition, 128(2), 179–186.
Wittgenstein, L. (1921/1922). Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd.
Wittgenstein, L. (1953/2010). Philosophical investigations. Blackwell Publishing.
Wnuk, E., & Majid, A. (2014). Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq. Cognition, 131(1), 125–138.
Xu, J., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. (2013). Cultural transmission results in convergence towards colour term universals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2801, 1758.
Xu, Y., & Regier, T. (2014). Numeral systems across languages support efficient communication: From approximate numerosity to recursion. In P. Bello et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Xu, Y., & Kemp, C. (2015) A computational evaluation of two laws of semantic change. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A.S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C.D. Jennings, & P.P. Maglio (Eds.), 37th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Xu, Y., Regier, T., & Malt, B.C. (2015), Historical semantic chaining and efficient communication: The case of container names. Cognitive Science.
Yee, E., & Thompson-Schill, S.L. (2016). Putting concepts into context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1015–1027.
Zipf, G. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. New York: Addison-Wesley.
Zuidema, W., & Westermann, G. (2003). Evolution of an optimal lexicon under constraints from embodiment. Artificial Life, 1–14.
Zwarts, J. (1995). The semantics of relative position. Proceedings of
Semantics and Linguistic Theory
, 51, 405–422.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Sun, Yue & David Poeppel
2023.
Syllables and their beginnings have a special role in the mental lexicon.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 120:36
Carr, Jon W., Kenny Smith, Jennifer Culbertson & Simon Kirby
2020.
Simplicity and informativeness in semantic category systems.
Cognition 202
► pp. 104289 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.