Research has shown that, in English, the mapping between a word’s form and its syntactic category is not entirely arbitrary. Though formal differences between lexical categories are subtle, adults are sensitive to them and access this knowledge when retrieving or manipulating grammatical category information. Studies of form typicality have so far exclusively investigated unambiguous (or disambiguated) wordforms. We test the prediction that form typicality also affects visual processing of ambiguous wordforms, with formal features correlating, not with a form’s designation as a particular category, but with a form’s probability of being used as a particular category. Our results indicate that “form discrepancy”, a measure of how well a form’s category usage matches up with its form (i.e. typically nouny forms associated with high probability of usage as a noun), is a significant predictor of lexical decision response time. These data are in line with models in which category is not specified for roots in the lexicon but rather assigned within syntactic or semantic context, and show that distributional information about grammatical category usage is automatically accessed in visual word processing.
Abdi, H. (2003). Factor Rotations in Factor Analyses. In M. Lewis-Beck, A. Bryman., & T. Futing (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Science Research (pp. 1–8). Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage.
Akaike, H. (1974). A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723.
Baayen, R. H. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (release 2). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data.
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459.
Belsley, D. A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R. E. (1980). Regression diagnostics: identifying sources of influential observations and collinearity. New York: Wiley.
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.
Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterising semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 531–544.
De Saussure, F. (1916). Nature of the linguistic sign. In Course in general linguistics, 65–70.
Cassidy, K. W., & Kelly, M. H. (1991). Phonological information for grammatical category assignments. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(3), 348–369.
Dikker, S., & Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Before the N400: Effects of lexical-semantic violations in visual cortex. Brain and Language, 1181, 23–28.
Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., & Pylkkänen, L. (2009). Sensitivity to syntax in visual cortex. Cognition, 110(3), 293–321.
Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., Farmer, T., & Pylkkänen, L. (2010). Early occipital sensitivity to syntactic category is based on form typicality. Psychological Science, 21(5), 629–634.
Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience and the English past tense: Comments on the paper by Ullman et al.Brain and language, 93(2), 243–247.
Farmer, T., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2006). Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(32), 12203–12208.
Farmer, T., Monaghan, P., Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Phonological typicality influences sentence processing in predictive contexts: Reply to Staub, Grant, Clifton, and Rayner (2009). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1318–1325.
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84.
Jurafsky, D., & Martin, J. (2008). Speech and language processing: An introduction to natural language processing, computational linguistics, and speech recognition. Prentice Hall.
Kaiser, H. F. (1958). The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 23(3), 187–200.
Kelly, M. H. (1992). Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. Psychological Review, 99(2), 349–364.
Kelly, M. H., & Bock, J. K. (1988). Stress in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 389–403.
King, J., Linzen, T., and Marantz, A. (in press). Syntactic categories as lexical features or syntactic heads: An MEG approach. [[URL]
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology (2nd edn.). New York: Liveright Publishing.
Linzen, T. (2015). Probabilistic linguistic representations: between learning and processing (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). New York University, New York City.
Lumley, T. using Fortran code by Alan Miller. (2009). leaps: regression subset selection. R package version 2.9. [URL]
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Marchand, H. (1969). The Categories and Types of Present-day English Word-formation (2nd Edition). Munich, Federal Republic of Germany: C.H. Beck’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung.
Mansfield, E. R., & Helms, B. P. (1982). Detecting multicollinearity. The American Statistician, 36(3a), 158–160.
Monaghan, P., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation. Cognition, 96(2), 143–182.
Morgan, J. L., Shi, R., &Allopenna, P. (1996). Perceptual bases of grammatical categories. In J. L. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 263–283.
O’Brien, R. M. (2007). A Caution Regarding Rules of Thumb for Variance Inflation Factors. Quality & Quantity, 41(5), 673–690.
R Core Team (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL].
Reilly, J., & Kean, J. (2007). Formal distinctiveness of high‐and low‐imageability nouns: Analyses and theoretical implications. Cognitive science, 31(1), 157–168.
Reilly, J., Hung, J., & Westbury, C. (2016). Non‐Arbitrariness in Mapping Word Form to Meaning: Cross‐Linguistic Formal Markers of Word Concreteness. Cognitive Science, 1–19.
Revelle, W. (2015). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA, [URL]Version = 1.5.8.
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of experimental psychology, 12(3), 225.
Sereno, J. A., & Jongman, A. (1990). Phonological and Form Class Relations in the Lexicon, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 19(6), 387–404.
Sherman, D. (1975). Noun-verb stress alternation: an example of the lexical diffusion of sound chance in English. Linguistics, 13(159), 43–72.
Shibata, R. (1981). An optimal selection of regression variables. Biometrika, 68(1), 45–54.
Spieler, D. H., & Balota, D. A. (1997). Bringing computational models of word naming down to the item level. Psychological Science, 8(6), 411–416.
Staub, A., Grant, M., Clifton, C., & Rayner, K. (2009). Phonological typicality does not influence fixation durations in normal reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 806–14.
Storkel, H. L., Armbruster, J., & Hogan, T. P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1175–1192.
Tabak, W., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Lexical statistics and lexical processing: semantic density, information complexity, sex, and irregularity in Dutch. In S. Kepser & M. Reis (Eds.), Linguistic evidence: Empirical, theoretical, and computational perspectives (pp. 529–555). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Tarkiainen, A., Helenius, P., Hansen, P. C., Cornelissen, P. L., & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Brain, 122(11), 2119–2131.
Turnbull, R., & Peperkamp, S. (2016, November). What governs a language’s lexicon? Determining the organizing principles of phonological neighbourhood networks. In International Workshop on Complex Networks and their Applications (pp. 83–94). Springer International Publishing.
Vitevitch, M. S. (2002). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 735.
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(3), 374–408.
Ziegler, J. C., Muneaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2003). Neighborhood effects in auditory word recognition: Phonological competition and orthographic facilitation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(4), 779–793.
Zucchini, W. (2000). An Introduction to Model Selection. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44(1), 41–61.
Cited by (7)
Cited by seven other publications
Kearney, Elaine, Katie L. McMahon, Frank Guenther, Joanne Arciuli & Greig I. de Zubicaray
2025. Revisiting the concreteness effect: Non-arbitrary mappings between form and concreteness of English words influence lexical processing. Cognition 254 ► pp. 105972 ff.
de Zubicaray, Greig I, Joanne Arciuli, Frank H Guenther, Katie L McMahon & Elaine Kearney
2024. Non-arbitrary mappings between size and sound of English words: Form typicality effects during lexical access and memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 77:5 ► pp. 943 ff.
Lukic, Sladjana, Alexandra Krauska, Masaya Yoshida & Cynthia K. Thompson
2023. The role of category ambiguity in normal and impaired lexical processing: can you paint without the paint?. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 17
Cassani, Giovanni & Niklas Limacher
2022. Not just form, not just meaning: Words with consistent form-meaning mappings are learned earlier. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 75:8 ► pp. 1464 ff.
Grestenberger, Laura & Itamar Kastner
2022. Directionality in cross-categorial derivations. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 7:1
Sharpe, Victoria, Samir Reddigari, Liina Pylkkänen & Alec Marantz
2019. Automatic access to verb continuations on the lexical and categorical levels: evidence from MEG. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 34:2 ► pp. 137 ff.
Kastner, Itamar, Liina Pylkkänen & Alec Marantz
2018. The Form of Morphemes: MEG Evidence From Masked Priming of Two Hebrew Templates. Frontiers in Psychology 9
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.