Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:2 (2017) ► pp.159180
References (49)
References
Abdi, H. (2003). Factor Rotations in Factor Analyses. In M. Lewis-Beck, A. Bryman., & T. Futing (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Science Research (pp. 1–8). Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1974). A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (release 2). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data.Google Scholar
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belsley, D. A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R. E. (1980). Regression diagnostics: identifying sources of influential observations and collinearity. New York: Wiley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterising semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 531–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Saussure, F. (1916). Nature of the linguistic sign. In Course in general linguistics, 65–70.Google Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., & Kelly, M. H. (1991). Phonological information for grammatical category assignments. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(3), 348–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dikker, S., & Pylkkänen, L. (2011). Before the N400: Effects of lexical-semantic violations in visual cortex. Brain and Language, 1181, 23–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., & Pylkkänen, L. (2009). Sensitivity to syntax in visual cortex. Cognition, 110(3), 293–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dikker, S., Rabagliati, H., Farmer, T., & Pylkkänen, L. (2010). Early occipital sensitivity to syntactic category is based on form typicality. Psychological Science, 21(5), 629–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience and the English past tense: Comments on the paper by Ullman et al. Brain and language, 93(2), 243–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farmer, T., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2006). Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(32), 12203–12208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farmer, T., Monaghan, P., Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Phonological typicality influences sentence processing in predictive contexts: Reply to Staub, Grant, Clifton, and Rayner (2009). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1318–1325.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, D., & Martin, J. (2008). Speech and language processing: An introduction to natural language processing, computational linguistics, and speech recognition. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Kaiser, H. F. (1958). The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 23(3), 187–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M. H. (1992). Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. Psychological Review, 99(2), 349–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M. H., & Bock, J. K. (1988). Stress in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 389–403.Google Scholar
King, J., Linzen, T., and Marantz, A. (in press). Syntactic categories as lexical features or syntactic heads: An MEG approach. [[URL]
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology (2nd edn.). New York: Liveright Publishing.Google Scholar
Linzen, T. (2015). Probabilistic linguistic representations: between learning and processing (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). New York University, New York City.Google Scholar
Lumley, T. using Fortran code by Alan Miller. (2009). leaps: regression subset selection. R package version 2.9. [URL]
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marchand, H. (1969). The Categories and Types of Present-day English Word-formation (2nd Edition). Munich, Federal Republic of Germany: C.H. Beck’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung.Google Scholar
Mansfield, E. R., & Helms, B. P. (1982). Detecting multicollinearity. The American Statistician, 36(3a), 158–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monaghan, P., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation. Cognition, 96(2), 143–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M. H., Farmer, T., & Fitneva, S. (2010). Measures of phonological typicality: Robust coherence and psychological validity. The Mental Lexicon, 5(3), 281–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan, J. L., Shi, R., &Allopenna, P. (1996). Perceptual bases of grammatical categories. In J. L. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 263–283.Google Scholar
O’Brien, R. M. (2007). A Caution Regarding Rules of Thumb for Variance Inflation Factors. Quality & Quantity, 41(5), 673–690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL].
Reilly, J., & Kean, J. (2007). Formal distinctiveness of high‐and low‐imageability nouns: Analyses and theoretical implications. Cognitive science, 31(1), 157–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J., Hung, J., & Westbury, C. (2016). Non‐Arbitrariness in Mapping Word Form to Meaning: Cross‐Linguistic Formal Markers of Word Concreteness. Cognitive Science, 1–19.Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (2015). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA, [URL] Version = 1.5.8.
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of experimental psychology, 12(3), 225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sereno, J. A., & Jongman, A. (1990). Phonological and Form Class Relations in the Lexicon, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 19(6), 387–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sherman, D. (1975). Noun-verb stress alternation: an example of the lexical diffusion of sound chance in English. Linguistics, 13(159), 43–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shibata, R. (1981). An optimal selection of regression variables. Biometrika, 68(1), 45–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spieler, D. H., & Balota, D. A. (1997). Bringing computational models of word naming down to the item level. Psychological Science, 8(6), 411–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Staub, A., Grant, M., Clifton, C., & Rayner, K. (2009). Phonological typicality does not influence fixation durations in normal reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 806–14.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L., Armbruster, J., & Hogan, T. P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1175–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tabak, W., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Lexical statistics and lexical processing: semantic density, information complexity, sex, and irregularity in Dutch. In S. Kepser & M. Reis (Eds.), Linguistic evidence: Empirical, theoretical, and computational perspectives (pp. 529–555). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tarkiainen, A., Helenius, P., Hansen, P. C., Cornelissen, P. L., & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Brain, 122(11), 2119–2131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turnbull, R., & Peperkamp, S. (2016, November). What governs a language’s lexicon? Determining the organizing principles of phonological neighbourhood networks. In International Workshop on Complex Networks and their Applications (pp. 83–94). Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. (2002). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 735.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(3), 374–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Muneaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2003). Neighborhood effects in auditory word recognition: Phonological competition and orthographic facilitation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(4), 779–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zucchini, W. (2000). An Introduction to Model Selection. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44(1), 41–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (7)

Cited by seven other publications

Kearney, Elaine, Katie L. McMahon, Frank Guenther, Joanne Arciuli & Greig I. de Zubicaray
2025. Revisiting the concreteness effect: Non-arbitrary mappings between form and concreteness of English words influence lexical processing. Cognition 254  pp. 105972 ff. DOI logo
de Zubicaray, Greig I, Joanne Arciuli, Frank H Guenther, Katie L McMahon & Elaine Kearney
2024. Non-arbitrary mappings between size and sound of English words: Form typicality effects during lexical access and memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 77:5  pp. 943 ff. DOI logo
Lukic, Sladjana, Alexandra Krauska, Masaya Yoshida & Cynthia K. Thompson
2023. The role of category ambiguity in normal and impaired lexical processing: can you paint without the paint?. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 17 DOI logo
Cassani, Giovanni & Niklas Limacher
2022. Not just form, not just meaning: Words with consistent form-meaning mappings are learned earlier. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 75:8  pp. 1464 ff. DOI logo
Grestenberger, Laura & Itamar Kastner
2022. Directionality in cross-categorial derivations. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 7:1 DOI logo
Sharpe, Victoria, Samir Reddigari, Liina Pylkkänen & Alec Marantz
2019. Automatic access to verb continuations on the lexical and categorical levels: evidence from MEG. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 34:2  pp. 137 ff. DOI logo
Kastner, Itamar, Liina Pylkkänen & Alec Marantz
2018. The Form of Morphemes: MEG Evidence From Masked Priming of Two Hebrew Templates. Frontiers in Psychology 9 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.