Article published in:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 13:3 (2018) ► pp. 311332
References

References

Abel, B.
(2003) English idioms in the first language and second language lexicon: a dual representation approach. Second Language Research, 19(4), 329–358. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Al-Azary, H. & Buchanan, L.
(2017) Novel metaphor comprehension: Semantic neighbourhood density interacts with concreteness. Memory & Cognition, 45, 296–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C.
(2001) Characterizing semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 531–544. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cacciari, C., & Tabossi, P.
(1988) The comprehension of idioms. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(6), 668–683. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 328 ]
Caillies, S., & Butcher, K.
(2007) Processing of Idiomatic Expressions: Evidence for a New Hybrid View. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(1), 79–108. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caillies, S. & Declerq, C.
(2011) Kill the Song – Steal the Show: What Does Distinguish Predicative Metaphors From Decomposable Idioms? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40(3), 205–223. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carrol, G., Littlemore, J. & Dowens, M. G.
(2018) Of false friends and familiar foes: Comparing native and non-native understanding of figurative phrases. Lingua, 204, 21–44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Citron, F. & Goldberg, A.
(2014) Metaphorical sentences are more emotionally engaging than their literal counterparts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(1), 2585–2595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Citron, F., Cacciari, C., Kucharski, M., Beck, L., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A.
(2016) When emotions are expressed figuratively: Psycholinguistic and affective norms of 619 idioms for German (PANIG). Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 91–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M.
(1981) The MRC Psycholinguistic Database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33(4), 497–505. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cortese, M. J. & Fugett, A.
(2004) Imageability ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 384–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cronk, B. & Schweigert, W.
(1992) The comprehension of idioms: The effects of familiarity, literalness, and usage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13(2), 131–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A. & Carreiras, M.
(2008) NoA’s ark: Influence of the number of associates in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), pp. 1072–1077. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fox, J. & Weisberg, S.
(2018) Visualizing Fit and Lack of Fit in Complex Regression Models with Predictor Effect Plots and Partial Residuals. Journal of Statistical Software, 87(9), 1–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J. & Logie, R. H.
(1980) Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words. Behavior research methods & instrumentation, 12(4), 395–427. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamblin, J., & Gibbs, J. R. W.
(1999) Why You Can’t Kick the Bucket as You Slowly Die: Verbs in Idiom Comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(1), 25–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harris, R. J., Friel, B. M. & Mickelson, N. R.
(2006) Attribution of discourse goals for using concrete- and abstract-tenor metaphors with or without discourse context. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(6), 863–879. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Juhasz, B. J. & Yap, M. J.
(2013) Sensory experience ratings for over 5,000 mono-and disyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods, 45(1), 160–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Katz, A. N.
(1992) Psychological studies in metaphor processing: Extensions to the placement of terms in semantic space. Poetics Today, 13(4), 607–632. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B. & Bly, B.
(1995) Intuitions of the transparency of idioms: can one keep a secret by spilling the beans? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 89–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 329 ]
(1999) Swimming against the current: do idioms reflect conceptual structure? J. Pragmat. 31, 1559–1578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1998) Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(2), 257–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kounios, A., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Fleck, J. I., Grondin, R. & McRae, K.
(2009) Semantic richness and the activation of concepts in semantic memory: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1282, 95–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P. & Vigliocco, G.
(2009) Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112(3), pp. 473–481. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. & Christensen, R. H. B.
(2017) lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Libben, M. R. & Titone, D. A.
(2008) The multidetermined nature of idiom processing. Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1103–1121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, R. A. G., & Gibbs, R. W.
(1987) Processing idioms with multiple meanings. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16(1), 63–81.Google Scholar
Ojha, A. & Indurkhya, B.
(2016) On the role of perceptual features in metaphor comprehension. In E. Gola & F. Ervas (Eds.), Metaphor and Communication (pp. 147–170). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, A. & Clark, A. J.
(1986) The role of topic and vehicle imagery in metaphor comprehension. Communication and Cognition 19(3–4), 367–387.Google Scholar
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C. & Madigan, S. A.
(1968) Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of experimental psychology, 76(1p2), 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Caporali, A.
(2007) Testing idiom comprehension in aphasic patients: the effects of task and idiom type. Brain and Language, 100(2), 208–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Genoni, A.
(2004) The role of syntactic competence in idiom comprehension: a study on aphasic patients. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17(5), 371–382. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Lucchelli, F., Muggia, S., & Rizzo, S.
(2003) Idiom comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease: the role of the central executive. Brain, 126, 2419–2430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P., Hargreaves, I., Siakaluk, P., Bodner, G. & Pope, J.
(2008) There are many ways to be rich: Effects of three measures of semantic richness on visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15,(1), 161–167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL: http://​www​.R​-project​.org/
Rassiga, C., Lucchelli, F., Crippa, F., & Papagno, C.
(2009) Ambiguous idiom comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(4), 402–411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M.
(2002) 358,534 nonwords: The ARC Nonword Database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1339–1362. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 330 ]
Rodd, J. M.
(2004) The effect of semantic ambiguity on reading aloud: A twist in the tale. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(3), 440–445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schweigert, W.
(1986) The comprehension of familiar and less familiar idioms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 33–45. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1991) The Muddy Waters of Idiom Comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20(4), 305–314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siakaluk, P. D., Pexman, P. M., Aguilera, L., Owen, W. J. & Sears, C. R.
(2008) Evidence for the activation of sensorimotor information during visual word recognition: The body-object interaction effect. Cognition, 106(1), 433–443. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Rabanus, S., & Rösler, F.
(2007) Processing Verbs in German Idioms: Evidence Against the Configuration Hypothesis. Metaphor and Symbol, 22(3), 213–231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sprenger, S., Levelt, W., & Kempen, G.
(2006) Lexical access during the production of idiomatic phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(2), 161–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D., & Connine, C.
(1994) Comprehension of Idiomatic Expressions: Effects of Predictability and Literality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20(5), 1126–1138.Google Scholar
(1999) On the compositional and noncompositional nature of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1655–1674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D., & Libben, M.
(2014) Time-dependent effects of decomposability, familiarity and literal plausibility on idiom meaning activation: A cross-modal priming investigation. The Mental Lexicon, 9(3), 473–496.Google Scholar
Toglia, M. P. & Battig, W. F.
(1978) Handbook of semantic word norms. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2014) Subtlex-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1176–1190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Lancker, D., Canter, G. J. and Terbeek, D.
(1981) Disambiguation of ditropic sentences: acoustic and phonetic cues. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 24(3), 322–329. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woollams, A. M.
(2005) Imageability and ambiguity effects in speeded naming: Convergence and divergence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(5), 878–890.Google Scholar
Xu, X.
(2010) Interpreting metaphorical statements. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 1622–1636. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yap, M. J., Tan, S. E., Pexman, P. M., & Hargreaves, I. S.
(2011) Is more always better? Effects of semantic richness on lexical decision, speeded pronunciation, and semantic classification. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 742–750. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yap, M. J., Pexman, P., Wellsby, M., Hargreaves, I. S., & Huff, M. J.
(2012) An Abundance of Riches: Cross-Task Comparisons of Semantic Richness Effects in Visual Word Recognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(72), 1–10.Google Scholar
Yap, M. J. and Seow, C. S.
(2014) The influence of emotion on lexical processing: Insights from RT distributional analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), pp. 526–533. Crossref[ p. 331 ]Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Carrol, Gareth
2021.  In Figurative Language – Intersubjectivity and Usage [Figurative Thought and Language, 11], Crossref logo
Carrol, Gareth & Jeannette Littlemore
2020. Resolving Figurative Expressions During Reading: The Role of Familiarity, Transparency, and Context. Discourse Processes 57:7  pp. 609 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 05 january 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.