Morphological generalization of Hebrew verb classes
An elicited production study in native and non-native speakers
The present work investigated how morphological generalization, namely the way speakers extend their knowledge to
novel complex words, is influenced by sources of variability in language and speaker properties. For this purpose, the study
focused on a Semitic language (Hebrew), characterized by unique non-concatenative morphology, and native (L1) as well as
non-native (L2) speakers. Two elicited production tasks tested what information sources speakers employ in verbal inflectional
class generalization, i.e., in forming complex novel verbs. Phonological similarity was tested in
Experiment 1 and argument structure in
Experiment 2. The analysis focused on the
two most common Hebrew inflectional classes,
Paal and
Piel, which also constituted the vast
majority of responses in the two tasks. Unlike the commonly found outcomes in Romance inflectional class generalization, the
results yielded, solely for Piel, a graded phonological similarity effect and a robust argument structure effect, i.e., more Piel
responses in a direct object context than without. The L2 pattern partially differed from the L1: (i) argument structure effect
for L2 speakers was weaker, and (ii) L2 speakers produced more Paal than Piel responses. The results are discussed within the
framework of rule-based and input-based accounts.
Article outline
- Generalization of Hebrew verb classes
- Morphological generalization in non-native (L2) speakers
- The present study
- Experiment 1
- Method
- Participants
- Simulation
- Materials
- Procedure
- Data analysis
- Results
- Experiment 2
- Method
- Participants
- Materials
- Procedure
- Data analysis
- Results
- Discussion
- Language-specific effects on morphological generalization
- Speaker-specific effects on morphological generalization
- Conclusions
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
-
References
References (78)
References
Agathopoulou, E., & Papadopoulou, D. (2009). Regularity patterns of the Greek past perfective: a psycholinguistic study. In G. K. Giannakis, Μ. Baltazani, G. I. Xydopoulos & Α. Tsangalidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th international conference on Greek linguistics, Ioannina (pp. 1–13).
Albright, A. (2002). Islands of reliability for regular morphology: Evidence from Italian. Language, 684–709.
Albright, A., & Hayes, B. (2003). Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition, 901, 119–161.
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself: Stems and inflectional classes. MIT press.
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 390–412.
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 31, 12–28.
Berent, I., Pinker, S., & Shimron, J. (1999). Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: Evidence for mental variables. Cognition, 721, 1–44.
Berent, I., & Shimron, J. (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the obligatory contour principle. Cognition, 641, 39–72.
Berman, R. A. (1993). Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 201, 641–669.
Berman, R. A. (1997). Modern Hebrew. In R. Hetzron (Ed.), The Semitic languages (pp. 312–333). New York: Routledge.
Bolozky, S. (1999a). Measuring productivity in word formation: The case of Israeli Hebrew. Brill.
Bolozky, S. (1999b). On the special status of the vowels a and e in Israeli Hebrew. Hebrew Studies, 401, 233–250.
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2015). Structure, form, and meaning in the mental lexicon: Evidence from Arabic. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 301, 955–992.
Brooks, P. J., Braine, M. D., Catalano, L., Brody, R. E., & Sudhalter, V. (1993). Acquisition of gender-like noun subclasses in an artificial language: The contribution of phonological markers to learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 321, 76–95.
Brovetto, C., & Ullman, M. T. (2005). The mental representation and processing of Spanish verbal morphology. In Eddington, D. (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the 7th hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 98–105). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Press.
Bybee, J. L., & Moder, C. L. (1983). Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 251–270.
Chen, L., Shu, H. U. A., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 161–174.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 3–42.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2018). Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 401, 693–706.
Culbertson, J., Gagliardi, A., & Smith, K. (2017). Competition between phonological and semantic cues in noun class learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 921, 343–358.
Culbertson, J., & Wilson, C. (2013). Artificial grammar learning of shape-based noun classification. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 2118–2123). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Cuskley, C., Colaiori, F., Castellano, C., Loreto, V., Pugliese, M., & Tria, F. (2015). The adoption of linguistic rules in native and non-native speakers: Evidence from a Wug task. Journal of Memory and Language, 841, 205–223.
Dawdy-Hesterberg, L. G., & Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2014). Learnability and generalisation of Arabic broken plural nouns. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 291, 1268–1282.
Deutsch, A. (2016). The separability of morphological processes from semantic meaning and syntactic class in production of single words: Evidence from the Hebrew root morpheme. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 451, 1–28.
Dewaele, J. M., & Véronique, D. (2001). Gender assignment and gender agreement in advanced French interlanguage: A cross-sectional study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 41, 275–297.
Dussias, P. E., Marful, A., Gerfen, C., & Molina, M. T. B. (2010). Usage frequencies of complement-taking verbs in Spanish and English: Data from Spanish monolinguals and Spanish-English bilinguals. Behavior Research Methods, 421, 1004–1011.
Dussias, P. E., & Scaltz, T. R. C. (2008). Spanish-English L2 speakers’ use of subcategorization bias information in the resolution of temporary ambiguity during second language reading. Acta Psychologica, 1281, 501–513.
Eddington, D. (2000). Analogy and the dual-route model of morphology. Lingua, 1101, 281–298.
Eddington, D. (2002). Dissociation in Italian conjugations: A single-route account. Brain and Language, 811, 291–302.
Farhy, Y., & Veríssimo, J. (2019). Semantic Effects in Morphological Priming: The Case of Hebrew Stems. Language and Speech, 621, 737–750.
Farhy, Y., Veríssimo, J., & Clahsen, H. (2018a). Universal and particular in morphological processing: Evidence from Hebrew. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 711, 1125–1133.
Farhy, Y., Veríssimo, J., & Clahsen, H. (2018b). Do late bilinguals access pure morphology during word recognition? A masked-priming study on Hebrew as a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 211, 945–951.
Fisher, C., Hall, D. G., Rakowitz, S., & Gleitman, L. (1994). When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth. Lingua, 921, 333–375.
Frigo, L., & McDonald, J. L. (1998). Properties of phonological markers that affect the acquisition of gender-like subclasses. Journal of Memory and Language, 391, 218–245.
Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. A. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 371, 58–93.
Hahn, U., & Nakisa, R. C. (2000). German inflection: Single route or dual route?. Cognitive Psychology, 411, 313–360.
Hahne, A., Mueller, J. L., & Clahsen, H. (2006). Morphological processing in a second language: Behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence for storage and decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 181, 121–134.
Hopp, H. (2013). Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 291, 33–56.
Itai, A., & Wintner, S. (2008). Language resources for Hebrew. Language Resources and Evaluation, 421, 75–98.
Ionin, T., & Montrul, S. (2010). The role of L1 transfer in the interpretation of articles with definite plurals in L2 English. Language Learning, 601, 877–925.
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 434–446.
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 603–634.
Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 571, 1–33.
Hudson Kam, C. L., & Newport, E. L. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 11, 151–195.
Kam, C. L. H., & Newport, E. L. (2009). Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology, 591, 30–66.
Krause, H., Bosch, S., & Clahsen, H. (2015). Morphosyntax in the bilingual mental lexicon: An experimental study of strong stems in German. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 371, 597–621.
Leikin, M., Schwartz, M., & Share, D. L. (2010). General and specific benefits of bi-literate bilingualism: a Russian-Hebrew study of beginning literacy. Reading and Writing, 231, 269–292.
Leiner, D. J. (2016). SoSci Survey (Version 2.6.00) [Computer software]. Available at [URL]
Levy, Y. (1983). The acquisition of Hebrew plurals: The case of the missing gender category. Journal of Child Language, 101, 107–121.
Matthews, C. (2010). On the nature of phonological cues in the acquisition of French gender categories: Evidence from instance-based learning models. Lingua, 1201, 879–900.
Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2017). Balancing Type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 941, 305–315.
McCarthy, C. (2008). Morphological variability in the comprehension of agreement: An argument for representation over computation. Second Language Research, 241, 459–486.
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 551, 381–401.
McDonald, J. L., & Roussel, C. C. (2010). Past tense grammaticality judgment and production in non-native and stressed native English speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 429–448.
Montrul, S. (2011). Morphological errors in Spanish second language learners and heritage speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 163–192.
Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M. T. (2010). Second language acquisition of gender agreement in explicit and implicit training conditions: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 601, 154–193.
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 241, 933–947.
Naigles, L. R. (1996). The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping. Cognition, 581, 221–251.
Naigles, L. G., & Kako, E. T. (1993). First contact in verb acquisition: Defining a role for syntax. Child Development, 641, 1665–1687.
Parodi, T., Schwartz, B. D., & Clahsen, H. (2004). On the L2 acquisition of the morphosyntax of German nominals. Linguistics, 669–706.
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules. New York, NY: Harper Perennial.
Pinker, S. (2013). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. MIT press.
Plag, I. (2000). Irregular past tense formation in English interlanguage. Language Use, Language Acquisition and Language History. (Mostly) Empirical Studies in Honour of Rüdiger Zimmermann, 134–149.
Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 81, 1–56.
Ramscar, M. (2002). The role of meaning in inflection: Why the past tense does not require a rule. Cognitive Psychology, 451, 45–94.
Reeder, P. A., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Distributional learning of subcategories in an artificial grammar: Category generalization and subcategory restrictions. Journal of Memory and Language, 971, 17–29.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & The PDP Research Group (Eds.). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol.21, pp.216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Say, T., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Words, rules and stems in the Italian mental lexicon. In S. Nooteboom, F. Weerman, & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Storage and computation in the language faculty (pp. 93–129). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Schuler, K. D., Reeder, P. A., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). The effect of Zipfian frequency variations on category formation in adult artificial language learning. Language Learning and Development, 131, 357–374.
Stavrakaki, S., & Clahsen, H. (2009). The perfective past tense in Greek child language. Journal of Child Language, 361, 113–142.
Steinhauer, K., White, E. J., & Drury, J. E. (2009). Temporal dynamics of late second language acquisition: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Second Language Research, 251, 13–41.
Traxler, M. J. (2005). Plausibility and verb subcategorization in temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 341, 1–30.
Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2011). Words with and without internal structure: What determines the nature of orthographic and morphological processing?. Cognition, 1181, 141–156.
Veríssimo, J., & Clahsen, H. (2014). Variables and similarity in linguistic generalization: Evidence from inflectional classes in Portuguese. Journal of Memory and Language, 761, 61–79.
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., & Leung, Y. K. I. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 105–133.
Wilson, M. P., & Garnsey, S. M. (2009). Making simple sentences hard: Verb bias effects in simple direct object sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 601, 368–392.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Marzi, Claudia & Vito Pirrelli
2023.
A discriminative information-theoretical analysis of the regularity gradient in inflectional morphology.
Morphology 33:4
► pp. 459 ff.
Clahsen, Harald & Anna Jessen
2021.
Morphological generalization in bilingual language production: Age of acquisition determines variability.
Language Acquisition 28:4
► pp. 370 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.