Is inhibition involved in the processing of opaque compound words?
A study of individual differences
We examined whether inhibition skills were recruited during the processing of compound words. Using an individual
differences perspective, we analyzed whether participants’ scores on the Stroop test predicted performance on lexical decision tasks
involving compound words varying in their level of semantic opacity. The results show that inhibition is involved in the comprehension of
fully opaque (e.g., hogwash) and fully transparent (e.g., blueberry) compound words, but we found no
evidence for such an effect in the comprehension of partially opaque compound words (e.g., strawberry,
jailbird).
Article outline
- Experiment 1
- Methods
- Materials
- Lexical decision task
- Inhibition task
- Procedure
- Lexical decision task
- Inhibition task
- Participants
- Results and discussion
- Lexical decision task
- Inhibition task
- Lexical decision and inhibition task
- Inhibition scores and reaction times on the lexical decision task
- Inhibition ratio and reaction times on the lexical decision task
- Experiment 2
- Methods
- Materials
- Lexical decision task
- Inhibition task
- Procedure
- Participants
- Results and discussion
- Lexical decision task
- Inhibition task
- Lexical decision and inhibition task
- Inhibition scores and reaction times on the lexical decision task
- Inhibition ratio and reaction times on the lexical decision task
- Comparing Experiment 1 and Experiment 2
- General discussion
- Notes
-
References
References (41)
References
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480–485.
Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10(1), 48–71.
Bisiach, E., Mini, M., Sterzi, R., & Vallar, G. (1982). Hemispheric lateralization of the decisional stage in choice reaction times to visual unstructured stimuli. Cortex, 18(2), 191–197.
Brooks, T. L., & Cid de Garcia, D. (2015). Evidence for morphological composition in compound words using MEG. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 91:215.
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kuçera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.
Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Toth, J. P. (2008). Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task. Memory & Cognition, 36(8), 1484–1494.
Durgin, F. H. (2000). The reverse Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(1), 121–125.
Engle, Kane & Tuholski. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (pp.102–134). London: Cambridge Press.
Enright, S. J., & Beech, A. R. (1990). Obsessional states: Anxiety disorders or schizotypes? An information processing and personality assessment. Psychological Medicine, 20(3), 621–627.
Frith, C. D. (1979). Consciousness, information processing, and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1341, 225–35.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2009). Constituent integration during the processing of compound words: Does it involve the use of relational structures? Journal of Memory and Language, 60(1), 20–35.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2014). Typing time as an index of morphological and semantic effects during English compound processing. Lingue e Linguaggio, 13(2), 241–262.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2016). Written production of English compounds: effects of morphology and semantic transparency. Morphology, 26(2), 133–155.
Gignac, G. E., & Vernon, P. A. (2004). Reaction time and the dominant and non-dominant hands: An extension of Hick’s Law. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(3), 733–739.
Glucksberg, S., Newsome, M., & Goldvarg, Y. (2001). Inhibition of the literal: Filtering metaphor-irrelevant information during metaphor comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 161, 277–293.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 22 (pp. 193–225). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention & Performance, XVII, Cognitive Regulation of Performance: Interaction of Theory and Application (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Horga, G., & Maia, T. V. (2012). Conscious and unconscious processes in cognitive control: a theoretical perspective and a novel empirical approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 61.
Iakimova, G., Passerieux, C., & Hardy-Bayle, M. C. (2006). The understanding of metaphors in schizophrenia and depression. An experimental approach. Encephale, 321, 995–1002.
Imbrosciano, A., & Berlach, R. (2006). The Stroop test and its relationship to academic performance and general behaviour of young students. Teacher Development, 91, 131–144.
Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2011). Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 651, 406–430.
Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 121, 128–139.
Juhasz, B. J. (2007). The influence of semantic transparency on eye movements during English compound word recognition. In R. von Gompel, W. Murray, & M. Fischer (Eds.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 373–389). Boston, MA: Elsevier.
Knight, S., & Heinrich, A. (2017). Different measures of auditory and visual stroop interference and their relationship to speech intelligibility in noise. Frontiers in Psychology, 81, 230.
Kuperman, V., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2011). Individual differences in visual comprehension of morphological complexity. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1643–1648). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Libben, G. (1993). A Case of obligatory access to morphological constituents. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 161, 111–12.
Libben, G. (2005). Everything is psycholinguistics: Material and methodological considerations in the study of compound processing. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics / La revue canadienne de linguistique. 501. 267–283.
Libben, G., Gibson, M., Yoon, Y. B., & Sandra, D. (2003). Compound fracture: The role of semantic transparency and morphological headedness. Brain and Language, 841, 50–64.
Logan, G. D., Zbrodoff, N. J., & Williamson, J. (1984). Strategies in the color-word Stroop task. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(2), 135–138.
MacGregor, L., & Shtyrov, Y. (2013). Multiple routes for compound word processing in the brain: Evidence from EEG. Brain & Language, 1261, 217–229.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 1091, 163–203.
Monterosso, J. R., Aron, A. R., Cordova, X., Xu, J., & London, E. D. (2005). Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79(2), 273–277.
Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York, NY: Springer.
Sandra, D. (1990). On the representation and processing of compound words: Automatic access to constituent morphemes does not occur. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 421, 529–567.
Sani, S. R., Tabibi, Z., Fadardi, J. S., & Stavrinos, D. (2017). Aggression, emotional self-regulation, attentional bias, and cognitive inhibition predict risky driving behavior. Accident; analysis and prevention, 1091, 78–88.
Seidenberg, M. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Leiman, J. M., & Bienkowski, M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations on knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 141, 489–532.
Schmidtke, D., Van Dyke, J. A., & Kuperman, V. (2018). Individual variability in the semantic processing of English compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(3), 421–439.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 181, 643–662.
Traverso, L., Mantini, C., Usai, M. C., & Viterbori, P. (2015). The relationship between inhibition and working memory in preschoolers: Evidence for different inhibitory abilities. In G. Airenti, B. Bara, G. Sandini, & M. Cruciani (Eds.). Proceedings of the EuroAsianPacific Joint Conference on Cognitive Science / 4th European Conference on Cognitive Science / 11th International Conference on Cognitive Science (pp. 48–53). Torino, Italy.
Treisman, A., & Fearnley, S. (1969). The Stroop test: Selective attention to colours and words. Nature, 2221, 437–439.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Lombard, Alizée, Marine Wauquier, Cécile Fabre, Nabil Hathout, Lydia-Mai Ho-Dac & Richard Huyghe
Park, Juana, Faria Sana, Christina L. Gagné & Thomas L. Spalding
2021.
Factors that Influence the Processing of Noun-Noun Metaphors.
Metaphor and Symbol 36:1
► pp. 20 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.