Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 16:1 (2021) ► pp.4968
References
Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using Ime4. Journal of Statistical Software, 671, 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., Garrard, P., & Hodges, J. R.
(2000) Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 381, 1207–1215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breitenstein, C., Jansen, A., Deppe, M., Foerster, A. F., Sommer, J., Wolbers, T., & Knecht, S.
(2005) Hippocampus activity differentiates good from poor learners of a novel lexicon. NeuroImage, 251, 958–968. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chandler, P. M.
(2017, July). A Spanish Vocabulary Levels Test. Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Association of Teachers of Spanish and Portuguese, Chicago, IL.
Davis, M., & Gaskell, M. G.
(2009) A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioral evidence. Philosophical transactions: Biological sciences, 3641, 3773–3800. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elgort, I., & Warren, P.
(2014) L2 vocabulary learning from reading: Explicit and tacit lexical knowledge and the role of learner and item variables. Language Learning, 641, 365–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., Graff, C., & Finch, B.
(2019) Contributions of episodic memory to novel Word learning. The Mental Lexicon, 141, 381–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., Lum, J. A. G., & Ullman, M. T.
(2018) Child first and adult second language are tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1151, 1487–1492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Pandza, N. B.
(2020) Contributions of semantic and contextual diversity to the word frequency effect in L2 lexical access. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 25–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, E., Gaskell, M. G., Weighall, A., & Henderson, L.
(2017) Consolidation of vocabulary during sleep: The rich get richer? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 771, 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jamieson, R. K., Avery, J. E., Johns, B. T., & Jones, M. N.
(2018) An instance theory of Semantic memory. Computational Brain & Behavior, 11, 119–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N., & Forster, K. I.
(2001) Cross-language priming asymmetries in lexical decision And episodic recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 441, 32–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., Ullman, M. T., & Corkin, S.
(2001) Bilateral medial temporal lobe damage does not affect lexical or grammatical processing: Evidence from amnesic patient H.M. Hippocampus, 111, 347–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koda, K., & Miller, R. T.
(2018) Cross-linguistic interaction in L2 word meaning inference In English as a foreign language. In H. K. Pae (Ed.), Writing systems, reading processes, and cross-linguistic influences: Reflections from the Chinese, Japanese and Korean languages (pp. 293–312). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laufer, B., & Nation, P.
(1999) A vocabulary-size test of controlled productive ability. Language Testing, 161, 33–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, S., & Pulido, D.
(2017) The impact of topic interest, L2 proficiency, and gender on EFL incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading. Language Teaching Research, 211, 118–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malone, J.
(2018) Incidental vocabulary learning in SLA: Effects of frequency, aural enhancement, and working memory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 401, 651–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McRae, K., & Jones, M. N.
(2013) Semantic memory. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 206–219). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nation, I. S. P.
(1990) Teaching and learning vocabulary. Boston, MA: Heinle and Heinle.Google Scholar
Papi, M.
(2018) Motivation as quality: Regulatory fit effects on incidental vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 401, 707–730. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulido, D.
(2003) Modeling the role of second language proficiency and topic familiarity in second language incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading. Language Learning, 531, 233–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The effects of topic familiarity and passage sight vocabulary on L2 lexical inferencing and retention through reading. Applied Linguistics, 281, 66–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) How involved are American L2 learners of Spanish in lexical input Processing tasks during reading?. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 311, 31–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulido, D., & Hambrick, D. Z.
(2008) The virtuous cycle: Modeling individual differences in L2 reading and vocabulary development. Reading in a Foreign Language, 201, 164–190.Google Scholar
R Core Team
(2015) Changes in R. R Journal, 71, 293–297.Google Scholar
Renoult, L., Irish, M., Moscovitch, M., & Rugg, M. D.
(2019) From knowing to remembering: The semantic-episodic distinction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 231, 1041–1057. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rott, S.
(2007) The effect of frequency of input-enhancements on word learning and text comprehension. Language Learning, 571, 165–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K.
(1986) Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 41, 360–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takashima, A., Bakker, I., van Hell, J., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J.
(2014) Richness of information about novel words influences how episodic and semantic memory networks interact during lexicalization. NeuroImage, 841, 265–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Interaction between episodic and semantic memory networks in the acquisition and consolidation of novel spoken words. Brain and Language, 1671, 44–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trahan, D. E., & Larrabee, G. J.
(1988) Continuous visual memory test: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2004) Contribution of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 921, 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The biocognition of the mental lexicon. In M. G. Gaskell, G. Altmann, & P. Bloom (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 267–286). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2014) The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 135–158). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2016) The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiological model of Language learning, knowledge, and use. In G. Hickok & S. A. Small (Eds.), The neurobiology of language (pp. 953–968). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T. & Pullman, M. Y.
(2015) A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 511, 205–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Webb, S.
(2008) The effects of context on incidental vocabulary learning. Reading in a Foreign Language, 201, 232–245.Google Scholar
Webb, S., & Chang, A. C. S.
(2015) How does prior word knowledge affect vocabulary learning progress in an extensive reading program?, Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 371, 651–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wickens, T. D.
(2002) Elementary signal detection theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Witzel, N. O., & Forster, K. I.
(2012) How L2 words are stored: The episodic L2 hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 381, 1608–1621.Google Scholar
Zhao, A., Guo, Y., Biales, C., Olszewski, A.
(2016) Exploring learner factors in second Language (L2) incidental vocabulary acquisition through reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 281, 224–245.Google Scholar