References (38)
References
AboHamed, L., Mashaqba, B., & Al-Khawaldeh, N. (2023). The impact of L2 English on choice perception, interpretation, and preference for L1 Arabic speakers. Language and Cognition, 15 (4), 834–853. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardel, C. & Falk, Y. (2012). The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn & J. Rothman (Eds.) Third language acquisition in adulthood (pp. 61–78). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Białek, M., & Fugelsang, J. (2019). No evidence for decreased foreign language effect in highly proficient and acculturated bilinguals: A commentary on Čavar and Tytus (2018). Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 40 (8), 679–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S. (2019). The auditory foreign-language effect of moral decision making in highly proficient bilinguals. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 40 (10), 865–878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2014). Emotionality differences between a native and foreign language: Theoretical implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 1, Article 1055. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Čavar, F., & Tytus, A. E. (2018). Moral judgement and foreign language effect: When the foreign language becomes the second language. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 39 (1), 17–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Flexas, A., Calabrese, M., Gut, N. K., & Gomila, A. (2014). Moral judgment reloaded: A moral dilemma validation study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 1, 6071. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Circi, R., Gatti, D., Russo, V., & Vecchi, T. (2021). The foreign language effect on decision-making: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28 1, 1131–1141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Corey, J. D., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Vives, M.-L., & Keysar, B. (2019). The role of intentions and outcomes in the foreign language effect on moral judgements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72 (1), 8–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Arnon, I., Aparici, M., & Apesteguia, J. (2014). ‘Piensa’ twice: On the foreign language effect in decision making. Cognition, 130 (2), 236–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Apesteguia, J., Heafner, J., & Keysar, B. (2014). Your morals depend on language. PLoS ONE, 9 (4), e94842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Angelis, G. (2007). Third or additional language acquisition. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Del Maschio, N., Crespi, F., Peressotti, F., Abutalebi, J., & Sulpizio, S. (2022). Decision-making depends on language: A meta-analysis of the Foreign Language Effect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25 (4), 617–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Del Maschio, N., Del Mauro, G., Bellini, C., Abutalebi, J., & Sulpizio, S. (2022). Foreign to whom? Constraining the moral foreign language effect on bilinguals’ language experience. Language and Cognition, 14 (4), 511–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M. (2010). “Christ fucking shit merde!” Language preferences for swearing among maximally proficient multilinguals. Sociolinguistic Studies, 4 (3), 595–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Driver, M. Y. (2020). Switching codes and shifting morals: How code-switching and emotion affect moral judgment. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25 (3), 905–921. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dylman, A. S., & Champoux-Larsson, M.-F. (2020). It’s (not) all Greek to me: Boundaries of the foreign language effect. Cognition, 196 1, Article 104148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41 1, 1149–1160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flexas, A., López-Penadés, R., Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., & Adrover-Roig, D. (2023). Meanness trumps language: Lack of foreign language effect in early bilinguals’ moral choices. PLoS ONE, 18 (11), e0294523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gao, F., Wu, C., Fu, H., Xu, K., & Yuan, Z. (2023). Language nativeness modulates physiological responses to moral vs. immoral concepts in Chinese-English bilinguals: Evidence from event-related potential and psychophysiological measures. Brain Sciences, 13 (11), 1543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gao, S., Zika, O., Rogers, R. D., & Thierry, G. (2015). Second language feedback abolishes the “hot hand” effect during even-probability gambling. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35 (15), 5983–5989. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gawinkowska, M., Paradowski, M. B. & Bilewicz, M. (2013). Second language as an exemptor from sociocultural norms: Emotion-Related Language Choice revisited. PLoS ONE, 8 (12), e81225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2015). The Foreign Language Effect on moral judgment: The role of emotions and norms. PLoS ONE, 10 (7), e0131529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition, 154 1, 34–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hadjichristidis, C., Geipel, J., & Keysar, B. (2019). The influence of native language in shaping judgment and choice. In N. Srinivasan (Ed.), Progress in brain research (pp. 253–272). Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (4), 613–628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Tannenbaum, D., Costa, A., Corey, J. D., & Keysar, B. (2017). Thinking more or feeling less? Explaining the foreign-language effect on moral judgment. Psychological Science, 28 (10), 1387–1397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B., Hayakawa, S. L., & An, S. G. (2012). The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological Science, 23 (6), 661–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kyriakou, A., Foucart, A., & Mavrou, I. (2023). Moral judgements in a foreign language: Expressing emotions and justifying decisions. International Journal of Bilingualism, 27 (6), 978–995. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kyriakou, A., & Mavrou, I. (2023). What language does your heart speak? The influence of foreign language on moral judgements and emotions related to unrealistic and realistic moral dilemmas. Cognition and Emotion, 37 (8), 1330–1348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, C., Wang, H., Timmer, K., & Jiao, L. (2022). The foreign language effect on altruistic decision making: Insights from the framing effect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25 (5), 890–898. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miozzo, M., Navarrete, E., Ongis, M., Mello, E., Girotto, V., & Peressotti, F. (2020). Foreign language effect in decision-making: How foreign is it? Cognition, 199 1, Article 104245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ogrodniczak, M. & Paradowski, M. B. (under review). The Foreign Language Effect in bilingual decision-making in real-life scenarios.
Plonsky, L. & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64 (4), 878–912. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Purpuri, S., Vasta, N., Filippi, R., Li, W., & Mulatti, C. (2024). Does language shape the way we think? A review of the foreign language effect across domains. International Journal of Bilingualism. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stankovic, M., Biedermann, B., & Hamamura, T. (2022). Not all bilinguals are the same: A meta-analysis of the moral foreign language effect. Brain and Language, 227 1, Article 105082. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teitelbaum Dorfman, F., Kogan, B., Barttfeld, P., & García, A. M. (2024). Bilinguals on the footbridge: The role of foreign-language proficiency in moral decision making. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The trolley problem. The Yale Law Journal, 94 (6), 1395–1415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar