Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon: Online-First ArticlesA psycholinguistic analysis of clinical list-learning tests
Neuropsychological assessments depend on language-based measures of cognitive functioning and the proper diagnosis
of certain disorders relies on patterns of impairment shown on these measures (Lezak et al., 2004). The current project was
motivated by the relative lack of literature integrating psycholinguistic experimental findings and clinical neuropsychological
research on tests of verbal memory, specifically list learning. It has been well documented that word-level characteristics impact
language processing and memory (see Yap & Balota, 2015 for a review). Therefore,
it is critical that neuropsychologists begin to understand how current measures can be confounded by the underlying lexical and
semantic characteristics of the stimuli and how, if used properly, those characteristics could aid in diagnostic specificity. The
current study examined the structure of popular list learning tests and analyzed the influence of several psycholinguistic
variables on the performance of healthy undergraduate participants. Results demonstrated that (1) age of acquisition, emotional
valence, semantic neighborhood density, and imageability predicted recall accuracy of items from neuropsychological tests and (2)
only one of the ten clinical test lists examined adequately controlled for these influential variables. Thus, clinicians could be
missing clinically relevant data by ignoring psycholinguistic contributions to patient performance.
Keywords: list learning tests, verbal learning, memory testing, neuropsychological assessment, lexical characteristics, semantic processing
Article outline
- Method
- Stimulus development
- Procedure
- Data analysis and results
- Discussion
-
References
Published online: 17 January 2025
https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.24023.lan
https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.24023.lan
References (35)
Angwin, A. J., Chenery, H. J., Copland, D. A., Murdoch, B. E., & Silburn, P. A. (2006). Self-paced
reading and sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of
Neurolinguistics,
19
(3), 239–252.
Brandt, J., & Benedict, R. H. B. (2001). Hopkins
verbal learning test — Revised. Administration manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Buchanan, L., Westbury, C., & Burgess, C. (2001). Characterizing
semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and
Review,
8
(3), 531–544.
Croisile, Ska B., Brabant, M.-J., Duchene, A., Lepage, Y., Aimard, G., & Trillet, M. (1996). comparative
study of oral and written picture description in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and
Language,
53
(1), 1–19.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1994). California
Verbal Learning Test, Children’s Version (CVLT-C). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
(2017). California
Verbal Learning Test, Third Edition (CVLT-3). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Durda, K., & Buchanan, L. (2006). WordMine2 [Online] Available: [URL]
Halderman, L. K., & Chiarello, C. (2005). Cerebral
asymmetries in early orthographic and phonological reading processes: Evidence from backward
masking. Brain and
Language
95
(2), 342–352.
Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2012). Semantic
diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of
words. Behavior Research
Methods,
45
(3), 718–730.
Huff, F. J., Corkin, S., & Growdon, J. H. (1986). Semantic
impairment and anomia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and
Language,
28
(2), 235–249.
Jenkins, J. J. (1979). Four
points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory
experiments. In Cermak, L. S., Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), Levels
of processing in human
memory (pp. 429–446). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY
— Second Edition (NEPSY — II) [Database record]. APA PsycTests.
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition
ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research
Methods,
44
(4), 978–990.
Lau, M. C., Goh, W. D., & Yap, M. J. (2018). An
item-level analysis of lexical-semantic effects in free recall and recognition memory using the megastudy
approach. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology,
71
(10), 2207–2222.
Lund, K., and Burgess, C. (1996). Producing
high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research
Methods,
28
1, 203–208.
Lutfallah, S., Fast, C., Rangan, C., & Buchanan, L. (2018). Semantic
neighbourhoods: There’s an app for that. The Mental
Lexicon,
13
(3), 388–393.
Lynott, D., Connell, L., Brysbaert, M., Brand, K., & Carney, J. (2020). The
Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: Multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English
words. Behavior Research
Methods,
52
1, 1271–291.
Madan, C. R. (2021). Exploring
word memorability: How well do different word properties explain item free-recall
probability? Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review,
28
(2), 583–595.
McNamara, P., Obler, L. K., Au, R., Durso, R., & Albert, M. L. (1992). Speech monitoring
skills in Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and normal aging. Brain and
Language,
42
(1), 38–51.
Mendez, M. F., Clark, D. G., Shapira, J. S., & Cummings, J. L. (2003). Speech
and language in progressive nonfluent aphasia compared with early Alzheimer’s
disease. Neurology,
61
(8), 1108–1113.
Milberg, W. P., Hebben, N., Kaplan, E., Grant, I., & Adams, K. (2009). The
Boston process approach to neuropsychological assessment. Neuropsychological assessment of
neuropsychiatric and neuromedical
disorders,
3
1, 42–65.
Oldfield, R. C. (1966). Things,
words, and the brain. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology,
18
(4), 340–535.
Price, C. C., & Grossman, M. (2005). Verb
agreements during on-line sentence processing in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal
dementia. Brain and
Language,
94
(2), 217–232.
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and
environment for statistical computing. (Version 4.1) [Computer
Software].
Randolph, C., Tierney, M. C., Mohr, E., & Chase, T. N. (1998). The
Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): Preliminary clinical
validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology,
20
(3), 310–319.
Scott, G. G., Keitel, A., Becirspahic, M., Yao, B., & Sereno, S. C. (2019). The Glasgow norms:
Ratings of 5,500 words on nine scales. Behavior Research
Methods,
51
(3), 1258–1270.
Sherman, E. M. S., Brooks, B. L. (2015). Child
and Adolescent Memory Profile (ChAMP). Psychological Assessment Resources.
Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (2003). Wide
range assessment of memory and learning (2nd ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A
compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (
3rd
ed.) Oxford University Press.
Taler, V., Davidson, P. S., Sheppard, C., & Gardiner, J. (2021). A
discourse-theoretic approach to story recall in aging and mild cognitive impairment. Aging,
Neuropsychology, and
Cognition,
28
(5), 762–780.
Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual
cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology,
12
1, 242–248.
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice:
Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical
Software,
45
(3), 1–67. [URL].
Vandenberghe, R. R., Vandenbulcke, M., Weintraub, S., Johnson, N., Porke, K., Thompson, C. K., & Mesulam, M. M. (2005). Paradoxical
features of word finding difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. Annals of
Neurology,
57
(2), 204–209.