Article In:
The Mental Lexicon: Online-First ArticlesSymbols to shapes processing
The Bouba/Kiki (BK) effect is observed when a linguistic sound is associated with a shape. People usually
associate the nonword bouba with a round shape, and kiki with a sharp shape (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001). In 2011, Nielsen and Rendall found that certain
English letters (/k/, /p/, and /t/) and (/b/, /l/, /m/, and /n/) were associated with sharp and round shapes respectively. The BK
effect was investigated in depth for the first time in Arabic in 2022 (Nassereddine) using Arabic Analogs to the English letters.
Arabic participants’ performance was not consistent with previous research (2011). The goal of the present study was to determine
the roundest and sharpest Arabic letters by presenting all letters both visually and to Arabic speakers and have them say whether
a letter shape or sound best maps on to the standard bouba and kiki shapes. The results revealed
that Arabic does have both round and sharp letters, and that there is a strong influence of phonological features on this BK
effect.
Keywords: Bouba/Kiki effect, Arabic, round and sharp letters
Article outline
- Method
- Participants
- Stimuli
- Letter stimuli
- Image stimuli
- Procedure
- Results
- Discussion
- Note
-
References
This content is being prepared for publication; it may be subject to changes.
References (14)
Bottini, R., Barilari, M., & Collignon, O. (2019). Sound
symbolism in sighted and blind. The role of vision and orthography in sound-shape
correspondences. Cognition,
185
1, 62–70.
Bremner, A. J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K. J., & Spence, C. (2013). “Bouba”
and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to
Westerners. Cognition,
126
(2), 165–172.
Cuskley, C., Simner, J., & Kirby, S. (2015). Phonological
and orthographic influences in the bouba-kiki effect. Psychological
research,
81
(1), 119–130.
Ćwiek, A., Fuchs, S., Draxler, C., Asu, E. L., Dediu, D., Hiovain, K., Kawahara, S., Koutalidis, S., Krifka, M., Lippus, P., Lupyan, G., Oh, G. E., Paul, J., Petrone, C., Ridouane, R., Reiter, S., Schümchen, N., Szalontai, Á., Ünal-Logacev, Ö., Zeller, J., Perlman M., & Winter B. (2022). The
bouba/kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society
B,
377
(1841), 20200390).
Fort, M., Martin, A., & Peperkamp, S. (2015). Consonants
are more important than vowels in the bouba-kiki effect. Language and
Speech,
58
(2), 247–266.
Javed, F. (2013). Arabic
and English phonetics: A comparative study. The Criterion: An International Journal in
English,
4
(4), 1–13.
Nassereddine, G. (2022). The
Bouba/Kiki Effect on Gendered Objects in the Arabic Language (Master’s thesis, University of Windsor
(Canada)).
Nielsen, A., & Rendall, D. (2011). The
sound of round: evaluating the sound-symbolic role of consonants in the classic Takete-Maluma
phenomenon. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie
expérimentale,
65
(2), 115.
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia--a
window into perception, thought and language. Journal of consciousness
studies,
8
(12), 3–34.
Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2015). What’s
in a name? Sound symbolism and gender in first names. PloS
one,
10
(5), e0126809.