Article published in:
Semantic Considerations of Lexical Processing
[The Mental Lexicon 9:1] 2014
► pp. 67106
References

References

Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D. A., & Quesada, J. F.. (
2006) Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science , 17, 814-823. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Besner, D., & Joordens, S.. (
1995) Wrestling with ambiguity—Further reflections: Reply to Masson and Borowsky (1995) and Rueckl (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 21, 515-519. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A.. (
2005) Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 17, 905-917. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bleasdale, F. A.. (
1987) Concreteness-dependent associative priming: Separate lexical organization for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 13, 582-594. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bransford, J. D., & McCarrell, N. S.. (
1974) A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (pp. 189-229). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Brown, W. P., & Ure, D. M. J.. (
1969) Five rated characteristics of. 650 word association stimuli. British Journal of Psychology , 60, 233-249. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colangelo, A., Buchanan, L., & Westbury, C.. (
2004) Deep dyslexia and semantic errors: A test of the failure of inhibition hypothesis using a semantic blocking paradigm. Brain and Cognition , 54, 232-234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colangelo, A., & Buchanan, L.. (
2005) Semantic ambiguity and the failure of inhibition hypothesis as an explanation for reading errors in deep dyslexia. Brain & Cognition , 57(1), 39-42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K.. (
2005) Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain , 128, 615-627. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crutch, S. J., Ridha, B. H., & Warrington, E. K.. (
2006) The different frameworks underlying abstract and concrete knowledge: Evidence from a bilingual patient with a semantic refractory access dysphasia. Neurocase , 12, 151-163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davis, C. J., & Perea, M.. (
2005) BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods , 37, 665-671. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deerwester, S., Dumais, S., Furnas, G., Landauer, T., & Harshman, R.. (
1990) Indexing by latent semantic analysis. Journal of the American Society for Information Science , 41, 391-407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeGroot, A. M. B.. (
1989) Representational aspects of Word imageability and Word frequency as assessed through Word association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 15, 824-845. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, S. A., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K.. (
1988) Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language , 27, 429-446. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M.. (
2009) Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition , 110, 284-292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Estévez, A.. (
1991) Estudio normativo sobre ambigüedad en castellano. Cognitiva , 3, 237-271.Google Scholar
Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J.. (
1996) Effects of polysemy in lexical decision and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 22, 1331-1356. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hino, Y., Lupker, S. J., & Pexman, P. M.. (
2002) Ambiguity and synonymy effects in lexical decision, naming, and semantic categorization tasks: Interactions between orthography, phonology, and semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 28, 686-713. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Howell, J. R., & Bryden, M. P.. (
1987). The effects of word orientation and imageability on visual half-field presentations with a lexical decision task. Neuropsychologia , 25, 527-538. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Quinlan, P., McDonald, S., & Altmann, G. T.. (
2006) Models of high-dimensional semantic space predict language-mediated eye movements in the visual world. Acta Psychologica , 121, 65-80.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Joordens, S., & Besner, D.. (
1994). When banking on meaning is not (yet) money in the bank: Explorations in connectionist modeling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 20, 1051-1062. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jorge-Botana, G., León, J. A., Olmos, R., & Hassan-Montero, Y.. (
2010). Visualizing polysemy using LSA and the predication algorithm. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology , 61(8), 1706-1724.Google Scholar
Jorge-Botana, G., Olmos, R., & Barroso, A.. (
2012) The construction-integration framework: A means to disminish bias in LSA-based call routing. International Journal of Speech Technology , 15(2), 151-164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2012). Gallito (Version 2.0.1) [NLP Software]. Retrieved from: http://​elsemantico​.es​/gallito20​/index​-eng​.html
Jorge-Botana, G., Olmos, R., & Barroso, A.
Gallito 2.0: A natural language processing tool to support research on discourse. In Proceedings of the twenty-third Annual Meeting of the Society for Text and Discourse, Valencia from 16 to 18 July 2013.
Kilgarriff, A.. (
1997) I don't believe in word senses. Computers and the Humanities , 31(2), 91-113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.. (
1998) The representation of knowledge in minds as machines. International Journal of Psychology , 33(6), 411-420. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., Patel, V. L., & Ericsson, K. A.. (
1999) The role of long term working memory in text comprehension. Psychologia , 42, 186-198.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.. (
2000) Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory. Psyhonomic Bulletin and Review , 7, 257-266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2001) Predication. Cognitive Science , 25, 173-202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Bowles, A.. (
2002) Metaphor comprehension: What makes a metaphor difficult to understand?. Metaphor and Symbol , 17, 249-262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.. (
2008) Symbol systems and perceptual representations. In M. de Vega, A. M. Glenberg & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp. 145-164). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Mangalath, P.. (
2011). The construction of meaning. Topics in Cognitive Science , 3, 346-370. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klein, D. E., & Murphy, G. L.. (
2001) The representation of polysemous words. Journal of Memory and Language , 45, 259-282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T.. (
1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of the acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review , 104, 211-240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P.. (
2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition , 114, 96-104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M.. (
2011) Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science , 3, 273-302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A.. (
2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science , 33, 51-73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M., & Hutchinson, S.. (
2012) Neurological evidence linguistic processes precedeperceptual simulation in conceptual processing. Frontiers in Psychology , 3, 1-11.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S., & Shillcock, R.. (
2001). Rethinking the word frequency effect: The neglected role of distributional information in lexical processing. Language and Speech , 44, 295-323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D., & Magnuson, J. S.. (
2008). Attractor dynamics and semantic neighborhood density: Processing is slowed by near neighbors and speeded by distant neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 34(1), 65-79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Millis, K., & Larson, M.. (
2008) Applying the construction-integration framework to aesthetic responses to representational artworks. Discourse Process , 45, 263-287. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nakov, P., Popova, A., & Mateev, P.. (
2001) Weight functions impact on LSA performance. In Euro Conference RANLP’2001 (Recent Advances in NLP) , Tzigov Chark, Bulgaria (pp. 187-193).
Paivio, A.. (
1986)  Mental representations . New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
1991)  Images in mind: The evolution of a theory . New York: Harvester-Wheasheaf.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., & Lupker, S. J.. (
1999) The impact of semantic ambiguity on visual word recognition: Do homophone and polysemy effects co-occur? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology , 53, 323-334. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J.. (
2004). Semantic ambiguity and the process of generating meaning from print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 30, 1252-1270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Hargreaves, I. S., Edwards, J. D., Henry, L. C., & Goodyear, B. G.. (
2007). Neural correlates of concreteness in semantic categorization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 19, 1407-1419. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Piercey, C. D., & Joordens, S.. (
2000) Turning an advantage into a disadvantage: Ambiguity effects in lexical decision versus reading tasks. Memory & Cognition , 28, 657-666. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Riordan, B., & Jones, M. N.. (
2011) Redundancy in perceptual and linguistic experience: Comparing feature-based and distributional models of semantic representation. Topics in Cognitive Science , 3, 303-345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rodd, J., Gaskell, G., & Marslen-Wilson, W.. (
2002). Making sense of semantic ambiguity: Semantic competition in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language , 46, 245-266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Samson, D., & Pillon, A.. (
2003). Concreteness effects in lexical tasks: Access to a mental image? Brain and Language , 87, 25-26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Harnishfeger, K. K., & Stowe, R. W.. (
1998) Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language , 27, 499-520. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J.. (
1983) Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychololgy, Learning and Memory and Cognition , 9, 82-102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stowe, R. W.. (
1989) Context availability and the processing of abstract and concrete words. Reading Research Quartely , 24, 114-126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Akin, C. E.. (
1994) Developmental trends in lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Reading Research Quarterly , 29, 251-263. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián, N., Martí, M. A., Carreiras, M. F., & Cuetos, F.. (
2000)  LEXESP, léxico informatizado del Español . Barcelona: Ediciones de la Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N.. (
1987) Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science , 237, 1317-1323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wandmacher, T..
2005How semantic is latent semantic analysis? In Proceedings of RECITAL'05 , Dourdan, France.
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Jorge-Botana, Guillermo, Ricardo Olmos & Vicente Sanjosé
2017. Predicting Word Maturity from Frequency and Semantic Diversity: A Computational Study. Discourse Processes 54:8  pp. 682 ff. Crossref logo
Martínez‐Huertas, José Á., Guillermo Jorge‐Botana & Ricardo Olmos
2021. Emotional Valence Precedes Semantic Maturation of Words: A Longitudinal Computational Study of Early Verbal Emotional Anchoring. Cognitive Science 45:7 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 january 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.