Article published in:
Words & Constructions: Language complexity in linguistics and psychology
Edited by Juhani Järvikivi, Pirita Pyykkönen-Klauck and Matti Laine
[The Mental Lexicon 9:2] 2014
► pp. 196231
References

References

Acha, J., & Perea, M.
(2008) The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: Evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 245–264 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) On the role of consonants and vowels in visual word recognition: Evidence with a letter search paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 423–438. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alvarez, C.J., Carreiras, M., & Taft, M.
(2001) Syllables and morphemes: Contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 545–555. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S.
(1996) Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 775–800. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D.A., & Chumbley, J.I.
(1984) Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word-frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barber, H., Vergara, M., & Carreiras, M.
(2004) Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. NeuroReport, 15, 545–548. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G.
(1996) Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551–565. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.H., Echallier, J.F., & Pernier, J.
(1999) ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 235–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bertram, R., Hyönä, J., & Laine, M.
(2011) Morphology in language comprehension, production and acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26 (4-6), 457–481. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bertram, R., Laine, M., & Karvinen, K.
(1999) The interplay of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity in lexical processing: Evidence form a morphologically rich language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 213–226. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bertram, R., Pollatsek, A., & Hyöna, J.
(2004) Morphological parsing and the use of segmentation cues.
Bertram, R., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R.H.
(2000) The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: The role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 489–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Besner, D., Coltheart, M., & Davelaar, E.
(1984) Basic processes in reading: Computation of abstract letter identities. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38, 126–134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Besner, D., Smith, M.C., & MacLeod, C.M.
(1990) Visual word recognition: A dissociation of lexical and semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 862–869. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C.
(1992) An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 132–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, H.E., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K.
(1989) The acquisition of parafoveal word information in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 85–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, J.S., Bub, D., & Arguin, M.
(1996) A characterization of the word superiority effect in a case of letter-by-letter surface alexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology,13, 415–441. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Alvarez, C.J., & de Vega, M.
(1993) Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–780. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & Perea, M.
(2005) Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 585–589. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., & Perea, M.
(2002) Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in the lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1228–1242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J.
(1997) Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 857–871. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Barber, H.
(2005) Early event related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1803–1817. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, M., Vergara, M., & Perea, M.
(2007) ERP correlates of transposed-letter similarity effects: Are consonants processed differently from vowels? Neuroscience Letters, 419, 219–224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) ERP correlates of transposed-letter priming effects: The role of vowels vs. consonants. Psychophysiology, 46, 34–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K.I.
(2007) Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 165–182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colé, P., Magnan, A., & Grainger, J.
(1999) Syllable-sized units in visual word recognition: Evidence from skilled and beginning readers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 507–532. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, M., Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A.M.
(2007) Phonology as the source of syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from French. Memory and Cognition, 35, 974–983. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dambacher, M., Kliegl, R., Hofmann, M., & Jacbos, A.M.
(2006) Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading. Brain Research, 1084, 89–103. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davis, C.J., Perea, M., & Acha, J.
(2009) Re(de)fining the orthographic neighbourhood: The role of addition and deletion neighbours in lexical decision and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1550–1570. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Moor, W., & Brysbaert, M.
(2000) Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths. Psychological Research, 63, 159–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., & Carreiras, M.
(2011) The relative position priming effect depends on whether letters are vowels or consonants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 37, 1143–1163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Dimitropoulou, M., Grainger, J., Hernández, J.A., & Carreiras, M.
(2012) Differential sensitivity of letters, numbers and symbols to character transpositions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1610–1624. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Kinoshita, S., Carreiras, M., & Norris, D.
(2011) Is morpho-orthographic decomposition purely orthographic? Evidence from masked priming in the same-different task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 509–529. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Molinaro, N., Laka, I., Estévez, A., & Carreiras, M.
(2009) N250 effects for letter transpositions depend on lexicality: Casual or causal? NeuroReport, 20, 381–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M.
(2007) Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a morpheme level? Evidence for morpho-orthographic decomposition. Cognition, 105, 691–703. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Does darkness lead to happiness? Masked suffix priming effects. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1002–1020. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlich, K., & Rayner, K.
(1983) Pronoun assignment and semantic integration during reading: Eye movements and immediacy of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 75–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J.
(1992) Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence form masked non-word priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 353–372. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1993) The time-course of orthographic and phonological code activation in the early phases of visual word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 119–122. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K., & Davis, C.
(1984) Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 680–698. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K., Davis, C., Schoknecht, C., & Carter, R.
(1987) Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human experimental psychology, 39, 211–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Guess, K.
(1996)  Effects of prime duration and visual degradation inmasked priming . Paper presented at the 37th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL.
Foxe, J.J., & Simpson, G.V.
(2002) Flow of activation form V1 to frontal cortex in humans: A framework for defining “early” visual processing. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 139–150. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R.
(1998) Towards a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
George, N., Evans, J., Fiori, N., Davidoff, J., & Renault, B.
(1996) Brain events related to normal and moderately scrambled faces. Cognitive Brain Research, 4, 65–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J.
(1990) Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L.
(1996) Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 623–647. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Granier, J.P., Farioli, F., Van Asche, E., & Van Heuven, W.L.B.
(2006) Letter position information and printed word perception: The relative-position priming constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 865–884. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Kiyonaga, K., & Holcomb, P.J.
(2006) The time course of orthographic and phonological code activation. Psychological Science, 17, 1021–1026. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W., Hammond, E.J., & Supramamian, S.
(1983) Letters and shapes: Developmental changes in search strategies. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 11–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W., & Meara, P.
(1987) The effects of script on visual search. Second Language Research, 3, 102–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guerrera, C., & Forster, K.
(2008) Masked form priming with extreme transposition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 117–142. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P.J., & Grainger, J.
(2006) On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1631–1643. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P.J., Grainger, J., & O´Rourke, T.
(2002) An electrophysiological study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on printed word perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 938–950. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., & Quinlan, P.T.
(1990) Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517–560. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutzler, F., Bergmann, J., Conrad, M., Kronbichler, M., Stenneken, P., & Jacobs, A.M.
(2004) Inhibitory effects of first syllable-frequency in lexical decision: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 372, 179–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutzler, F., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A.M.
(2005) Effects of syllable-frequency in lexical decision and naming: An eye-movement study. Brain and Language, 92, 138–152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Inhoff, A.W.
(1984) The stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 612–624. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Inhoff, A.W., & Rayner, K.
(1986) Parafoveal word processing during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word frequency. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 49–57.Google Scholar
Johnson, R., Perea, M., & Rayner, K.
(2007) Transposed-letter effects in reading: Evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 209–229. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kinoshita, S., & Norris, D.
(2009) Transposed-letter priming of prelexical orthographic representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 35, 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kreuger, L.
(1989) Detection of intraword and interword letter repretition: A test of the word unitization hypothesis. Memory and Cognition, 17, 48–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ktori, M., & Pitchford, N.J.
(2008) Effect of orthographic transparency on letter position encoding: A comparison of Greek and English monoscriptal and biscriptal readers. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 258–281. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laudanna, A., Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A.
(1992) Processing inflectional and derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 333–348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lavric, A., Clapp, A., & Rastle, K.
(2007) ERP evidence of morphological analysis from orthography: A masked priming study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 866–877. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, H.W., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A.
(2001) The relative contribution of consonants and vowels to word identification during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 189–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lima, S.D.
(1987) Morphological analysis in sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 84–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liversedge, S.P., & Findlay, J.M.
(2000) Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 6–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lupker, S., & Davis, C.J.
(2009) Sandwich priming: A method for overcoming the limitations of masked priming by reducing lexical competitor effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 618–639. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maurer, U., Zevin, J.Z., & McCandliss, B.D.
(2008b) Left-lateralized N170 effects of visual expertise in reading: Evidence from Japanese syllabic and logographic scripts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1878–1891. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCandliss, B.D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S.
(2003) The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 293–299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D.E., & Schvaneveldt, R.W.
(1971) Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Molinaro, N., Conrad, M., Barber, H., & Carreiras, M.
(2010) On the functional nature of the N400: Contrasting effects related to visual word recognition and contextual semantic integration. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 1–7. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J., Frank, T., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P.J.
(2007) Semantic transparency and masked phonological priming: An ERP investigation. Psychopsysiology, 44, 506–521. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Murray, W., & Kennedy, A.
(1988) Spatial coding in the processing of anaphor by good and poor readers: Evidence from eye movement analyses. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 40, 693–718. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neville, H., Mills, D.L., & Lawson, D.
(1992) Fractionating language: Different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 244–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Niswander, E., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K.
(2000) The processing of derived and inflected suffixed words during reading. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 389–420. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S.
(2008) Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference. Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 434–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Acha, J.
(2009) Does letter position coding depend on consonant/vowel status? Evidence with the masked priming technique. Acta Psychologica, 130, 127–137. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Carreiras, M.
(1998) Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighborhood frequency in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 134–144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Lupker, S.J.
(2003) Transposed letter confusability effects in masked form priming. In S. Kinoshita, & S.J. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: State of the art (pp. 97–120). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2004) Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 231–246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., Moret-Tatay, C., & Carreiras, M.
(2011) Facilitation vs. Inhibition in the masked priming same-different matching task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 2065–2079. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Pollatsek, A.
(1998) The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 767–779. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Rosa, E.
(2002) The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66 (3), 180–194. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peressotti, F., & Grainger, J.
(1999) The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 691–706. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C.A., & Bell, L.
(1991) Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 473–485. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P.N., Lupker, S.J., & Hino, Y.
(2002) The impact of feedback semantics in visual word recognition: Number of features effects in lexical decision and naming tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 542–549. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pitchford, N.J., Ledgeway, T., & Masterson, J.
(2008) Effect of orthographic processes on letter position encoding. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 97–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Drieghe, D., Stockall, L., & de Almeida, R.G.
(2010) The interpretation of ambiguous trimorphemic words in sentence context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17 (1), 88–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M.
(2005) Does conal prime CANAL more than cinal? Masked phonological priming effects in Spanish with the lexical decision task. Memory and Cognition, 33, 557–565. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M.H., & New, B.
(2004) The borht in my brother´s brotherl: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 1090–1098. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R.
(1981) A theory of order relations in perceptual matching. Psychological Review, 88, 552–572. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K.
(1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., & Duffy, S.A.
(1988) Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14, 191–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., & McConkie, G.W.
(1976) What guides a reader’s eye movements? Vision Research, 16, 829–837. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., McConkie, G.W., & Zola, D.
(1980) Integrating information across eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 206–226. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., & Binder, K.S.
(1998) Phonological codes and eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24, 476–497. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Sereno, S., & Raney, G.E.
(1996) Eye movement control in reading: A comparison of two types of models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1188–1200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Slattery, T.J., Drieghe, D., & Liversedge, S.P.
(2011) Eye movements and word skipping during reading: Effects of word length and predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 37 (2), 514–528. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., & Well, A.D.
(1996) Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 504–509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Well, A.D., & Pollatsek, A.
(1980) Asymmetry of the effective visual field in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 6, 537–544. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Well, A.D., Pollatsek, A., & Bertera, J.H.
(1982) The availability of useful information to the right of fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 537–550. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rouibah, A., & Taft, M.
(2001) The functional role of syllabic structure in French visual word recognition: The BOSS hypothesis revisited. Memory & Cognition, 29, 373–381. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schendan, H.E., Giorgio, G., & Kutas, M.
(1998) Neurophysiological evidence for visual perceptual categorization of words and faces within 150ms. Psychophysiology, 35, 240–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J.
(2004) Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 333–367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Segui, J., & Grainger, J.
(1990) Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 65–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sereno, S.C., & Rayner, K.
(2000) Spelling-sound regularity effects on eye fixations in reading. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 402–409. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M.
(2001) Processing of orthographic structure by adults of different reading ability. Language and Speech, 44, 351–376. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Radeau, M.
(1995) The influence of the phonological characteristics of a language on the functional units of reading: A study in French. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 330–346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M.
(1990) Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory & Cognition, 18, 380–393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Zandt, T., Colonius, H., & Proctor, R.W.
(2000) A comparison of two response-time models applied to perceptual matching. Pscychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 208–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vergara-Martínez, M., Perea, M., Marín, A., & Carreiras, M.
(2011) The processing of consonants and vowels during letter identity and letter position assignment in visual-word recognition: An ERP study. Brain & Language, 118, 105–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
White, S.J., Rayner, K., & Liversedge, S.P.
(2005) The influence of parafoveal word length and contextual constraint on fixation durations and word skipping in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 466–471. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J.C., & Goswami, U.
(2005) Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J.C., & Jacobs, A.M.
(1995) Phonological information provides early sources of constraint in the processing of letter strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 567–593. 
 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Kastner, Itamar, Liina Pylkkänen & Alec Marantz
2018. The Form of Morphemes: MEG Evidence From Masked Priming of Two Hebrew Templates. Frontiers in Psychology 9 Crossref logo
Rakhlin, Natalia V., Catalina Mourgues, Cláudia Cardoso-Martins, Alexander N. Kornev & Elena L. Grigorenko
2019. Orthographic processing is a key predictor of reading fluency in good and poor readers in a transparent orthography. Contemporary Educational Psychology 56  pp. 250 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 august 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.