Article published In:
Bridging the Methodological Divide: Linguistic and psycholinguistic approaches to formulaic language
Edited by Stefanie Wulff and Debra Titone
[The Mental Lexicon 9:3] 2014
► pp. 437472
References
Akaike, H
(1974) A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Cohen Priva, U
(2013) More than words: The effect of multi-word frequency and constituency on phonetic duration. Language and Speech, 56(3), 349–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Snider, N
(2010) More than words: frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(1), 67–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R.H
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R.H., Hendrix, P., & Ramscar, M
(2013) Sidestepping the combinatorial explosion: An explanation of n-gram frequency effects based on naive discriminative learning. Language and Speech, 56(3), 329–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R.H., Milin, P., Djurdjevic, D., Hendrix, P., & Marelli, M
(2011) An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review, 118(3), 438–481. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bar, M
(2007) The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., & Bolker, B
(2011) lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Retrieved from [URL].
Battig, W., & Montague, W
(1969) Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 801, 1–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G., & Butterworth, B
(1979) Contextual probability and word frequency as determinants of pauses and errors in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 22(3), 201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belsley, D.A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R.E
(2004) Regression diagnostics: Identifying influential data and sources of collinearity. Hoboken, NJ, USA: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
Block, C., & Baldwin, C
(2010) Cloze probability and completion norms for 498 sentences: Behavioral and neural validation using event-related potentials. Behavior research methods, 42(3), 665–670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P., & Fischler, I
(1980) Completion norms for 329 sentence contexts. Memory and Cognition, 8(6), 631–642. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bormuth, J
(1966) Readability: A new approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 11, 79–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brants, T., & Franz, A
(2006) Web 1T 5-gram version 1. Philadelphia, PA USA: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.M
(1992) Linear models. In J.M. Chambers & T.J. Hastie (Eds.), Statistical models in S (Chap. 4). USA, NY: Wadsworth & Brooks.Google Scholar
Chou, Y.M., Polansky, A.M., & Mason, R.L
(1998) Transforming non-normal data to normality in statistical process control. Journal of Quality Technology, 30(2), 133–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conway, C.M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S., & Pisoni, D
(2010) Implicit statistical learning in language processing: word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114(3), 356–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Criss, A., Aue, W., & Smith, L
(2010) The effects of word frequency and context variability in cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(2), 119–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crowe, S
(1998) Decrease in performance on the verbal fluency test as a function of time: Evaluation in a young healthy sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(3), 391–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K., Urbach, T., & Kutas, M
(2005) Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dilkina, K., McClelland, J.L., & Plaut, D.C
(2010) Are there mental lexicons? The role of semantics in lexical decision. Brain Research, 13651, 66–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, W
(1999) A source book of Gestalt psychology. London, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Elman, J
(2011) Lexical knowledge without a lexicon? The Mental Lexicon, 6(1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fano, R.M., & Hawkins, D
(1961) Transmission of information: A statistical theory of communications. American Journal of Physics, 291, 793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillenbaum, S., Jones, L., & Rapoport, A
(1963) The predictability of words and their grammatical classes as a function of rate of deletion from a speech transcript1. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2(2), 186–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finn, P
(1977) Word frequency, information theory, and cloze performance: A transfer feature theory of processing in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 13(4), 508–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Francis, W., & Kucera, H
(1982) Frequency analysis of English usage. Boston, MA, USA: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Frank, S.L., & Bod, R
(2011) Insensitivity of the human sentence-processing system to hierarchical structure. Psychological Science, 22(6), 829–834. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z., & Bock, K
(1998) Constraint, word frequency, and the relationship between lexical processing levels in spoken word production. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(3), 313–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hahn, L.W., & Sivley, R.M
(2011) Entropy, semantic relatedness and proximity. Behavior Research Methods, 43(3), 746–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hay, J., Pelucchi, B., Estes, K., & Saffran, J
(2011) Linking sounds to meanings: Infant statistical learning in a natural language. Cognitive Psychology, 63(2), 93–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y
(2008) Anticipatory processes in sentence processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kučera, H., & Francis, W
(1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Dartmouth, NH, USA: Dartmouth Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S
(1984) Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McEvoy, C.L., Nelson, D.L., & Komatsu, T
(1999) What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of inter item associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(5), 1177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKenna, M.C
(1986) Cloze procedure as a memory-search process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 781, 433–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D., Graf Estes, K., & Magnuson, J
(2010) Computational modeling of statistical learning: Effects of transitional probability versus frequency and links to word learning. Infancy, 15(5), 471–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D.L., McEvoy, C.L., & Dennis, S
(2000) What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887–899. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, D.L., McEvoy, C.L., & Schreiber, T.A
(1998) The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. [URL].
Nelson, D.L., McKinney, V., Gee, N., & Janczura, G
(1998) Interpreting the influence of implicitly activated memories on recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 105(2), 299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S
(2008) Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 434–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Owens, M., O’Boyle, P., McMahon, J., Ming, J., & Smith, F
(1997) A comparison of human and statistical language model performance using missing-word tests. Language and Speech, 40(4), 377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Garrod, S
(2007) Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team
(2009) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., & Gitcho, N
(2007) Developmental change and the nature of learning in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(7), 274–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruff, R., Light, R., Parker, S., & Levin, H
(1997) The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57(3), 394–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N., & Newport, E.L
(1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 2741, 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P., & LaCount, K
(1988) Semantic relatedness and the scope of facilitation for upcoming words in sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(2), 344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shannon, C.E
(1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 271, 379–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1951) Prediction and entropy of printed English. Bell System Technical Journal, 30(1), 50–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaoul, C., & Westbury, C.F
(2011) Formulaic sequences: Do they exist and do they matter? The Mental Lexicon, 6(1), 171–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaoul, C., Westbury, C.F., & Baayen, R.H
(2013) The subjective frequency of word n-grams. Psihologija, 46(4), 497–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, N.J
(2011) Scaling up psycholinguistics. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation Downloaded in December, 2013 from [URL]. San Diego, CA, USA: University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Smith, N.J., & Levy, R
(2011) Cloze but no cigar: The complex relationship between cloze, corpus, and subjective probabilities in language processing. In Proceedings of the 33rd annual meeting of the cognitive science conference (pp. 1637–1642).
Sprenger, S., & van Rijn, H
Taylor, W
(1953) “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly, 30(4), 415–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., & Tucker, B.V
Willems, R., & Hagoort, P
(2007) Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review. Brain and Language, 101(3), 278–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, S
(2006) Generalized additive models: An introduction with R. USA, NY: CRC Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 11 other publications

Baayen, R. Harald, Petar Milin & Michael Ramscar
2016. Frequency in lexical processing. Aphasiology 30:11  pp. 1174 ff. DOI logo
Bejar, Isaac I., Paul D. Deane, Michael Flor & Jing Chen
2017. Evidence of the Generalization and Construct Representation Inferences for the GRE® revised General Test Sentence Equivalence Item Type. ETS Research Report Series 2017:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Hofmann, Markus J., Steffen Remus, Chris Biemann, Ralph Radach & Lars Kuchinke
2022. Language Models Explain Word Reading Times Better Than Empirical Predictability. Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence 4 DOI logo
Hollis, Geoff
2019. Learning about things that never happened: A critique and refinement of the Rescorla-Wagner update rule when many outcomes are possible. Memory & Cognition 47:7  pp. 1415 ff. DOI logo
Jacobs, Cassandra L.
2021. Quantifying Context With and Without Statistical Language Models. In Handbook of Cognitive Mathematics,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Jacobs, Cassandra L.
2022. Quantifying Context With and Without Statistical Language Models. In Handbook of Cognitive Mathematics,  pp. 1053 ff. DOI logo
Jacobs, Cassandra L., Gary S. Dell, Aaron S. Benjamin & Colin Bannard
2016. Part and whole linguistic experience affect recognition memory for multiword sequences. Journal of Memory and Language 87  pp. 38 ff. DOI logo
Lõo, Kaidi, Juhani Järvikivi, Fabian Tomaschek, Benjamin V. Tucker & R. Harald Baayen
2018. Production of Estonian case-inflected nouns shows whole-word frequency and paradigmatic effects. Morphology 28:1  pp. 71 ff. DOI logo
Manshu, Tu & Zhao Xuemin
2019. CCHAN: An End to End Model for Cross Domain Sentiment Classification. IEEE Access 7  pp. 50232 ff. DOI logo
Matusevych, Yevgen, Afra Alishahi & Ad Backus
2016. Modelling verb selection within argument structure constructions. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31:10  pp. 1215 ff. DOI logo
Wang, Guanfang, Xianshan Chen, Geng Tian, Jiasheng Yang & Huiling Chen
2022. A Novel N -Gram-Based Image Classification Model and Its Applications in Diagnosing Thyroid Nodule and Retinal OCT Images. Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine 2022  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.