Article published In:
Real Fictions: Fictionality, factuality and narrative strategies in contemporary storytelling
Edited by Sam Browse, Alison Gibbons and Mari Hatavara
[Narrative Inquiry 29:2] 2019
► pp. 293312
References (47)
References
Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, L. (1999), Beliefs in conspiracies, Political Psychology 201: 637–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barton, S. B., & Sanford, A. J. (1993). A case study of anomaly detection: Shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment. Memory & cognition, 21(4), 477–487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bjerg, O. & Presskorn-Thygesen, T. (2017). Conspiracy theory: Truth claim or language game?. Theory, Culture and Society, 34(1), 137–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brotherton, R., & French, C. C. (2014). Belief in conspiracy theories and susceptibility to the conjunction fallacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 281, 238–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). ‘Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire’, Frontiers in Psychology 41: 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coady, D. (2003). Conspiracy theories and official stories. International Journal of Applied Philosophy, 17(2), 197–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Conspiracy theories: The philosophical debate. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
(2007). Are conspiracy theories irrational?. Episteme, 4(2), 193–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohn, D. (1990). Signposts of fictionality. Poetics Today, 11(4), 755–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999). The distinction of fiction. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M. & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual impact of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. Journal of Social Psychology, 1481, 210–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmott, C. & Alexander, M. (2014). Foregrounding, burying and plot construction. In P. Stockwell & S. Whiteley (Eds.) The Cambridge Handbook of Stylistics (pp.329–343). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbons, A. (2012). Multimodality, cognition and experimental literature. London/New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giovanelli, M. & Mason, J. (2018). The language of literature: An introduction to stylistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 151, 731–742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2013). The effect of high-anxiety situations on conspiracy thinking. Current Psychology, 321, 100–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamburger, K. (1993). The logic of literature (2nd ed.; M. J. Rose, Trans.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Hatavara, M. & Mildorf, J. (2017). Hybrid fictionality and vicarious narrative experience. Narrative, 25(1), 65–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heaps, C. & Nash, M. (2001). Comparing recollective experiences in true and false autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 271, 920–930.Google Scholar
Iversen, S. & Nielsen, H. S. (2016). The politics of fictionality in documentary form: The act of killing and The ambassador . European Journal of English Studies, 20(3), 249–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kearns, E. M., Conlon, B. & Young, J. K. (2014). Lying about terrorism. Studies in Conflict and Terrorism, 37(5), 422–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the black english vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Leman, P. J., & Cinnirella, M. (2013). Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. E. (2013). NASA faked the moon landing – Therefore, (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of science. Psychological Science, 241, 622–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. & Pickerell, J. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 251, 720–725. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, H. S., Phelan, J. & Walsh, R. (2015). Ten theses about fictionality. Narrative, 23(1), 61–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pace, L. A. & Livingston, M. M. (2005). Protecting human subjects in internet research. Electronic Journal of Business Ethics and Organization Studies, 10(1), 35–41.Google Scholar
Perry, B. & Szalavitz, M. (2006). The Boy Who Was Raised as a Dog: And Other Stories from a Child Psychiatrist’s Notebook. USA: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pigden, C. (1995). Popper revisited, or what is wrong with conspiracy theories?. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 25(1), 3–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007). Conspiracy theories and the conventional wisdom. Episteme, 4(2), 219–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phelan, J. (2016). Local fictionality within global non-fiction: Roz Chast’s Why can’t we talk about something more pleasant? Enthymema, 161, 19–31.Google Scholar
Porter, S., Yuille, J. C. & Lehman, D. R. (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: implications for the recovered memory debate. Law and Human Behavior, 23(5), 517–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, L. (1938). Literature as exploration. New York: D. Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
(1978). The reader, the text, the poem: The transactional theory of the literary work. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Sanford, A. (2002). Context, attention, and depth of processing during interpretation. Mind and Language, 17(1–2), 188–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanford, A. & Emmott, C. (2014). Mind, brain and narrative. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sanford, A. & Sturt, P. (2002). Depth of processing in language comprehension: Not noticing the evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 382–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott Terry, W. (2009). Learning and memory: basic principles, processes and procedures. (4th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education Inc.Google Scholar
Spilioti, T. & Tagg, C. (2017). The ethics of online research methods in applied linguistics. Applied Linguistics Review, 8(2–3), 163–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stockwell, P. (2002). Cognitive poetics: An introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2009). Texture: A cognitive aesthetics of reading. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 1331, 572–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. S. (2012). Dead and alive: Belief in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 3(6), 767–773. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Prooijen, J. W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 431, 109–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VICE. (2018). The rise of the crisis actor conspiracy movement [video file]. Retrieved from [URL]
Cited by (5)

Cited by five other publications

Biar, Liana de Andrade, Naomi Orton & Liliana Cabral Bastos
2021. Tales from the South. Narrative Inquiry 31:1  pp. 126 ff. DOI logo
Bonetto, Eric & Thomas Arciszewski
2021. The Creativity of Conspiracy Theories. The Journal of Creative Behavior 55:4  pp. 916 ff. DOI logo
Orton, Naomi
2021. “MYTHS”, “TRUTHS” AND THE ROLE OF APPLIED LINGUISTICS IN CONTEMPORARY SOCIETY. Trabalhos em Linguística Aplicada 60:2  pp. 455 ff. DOI logo
Statham, Simon
2020. The year’s work in stylistics 2019. Language and Literature: International Journal of Stylistics 29:4  pp. 454 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.