Cited by

Cited by 9 other publications

Dickey, Michael Walsh & Ann C. Bunger
2011. Comprehension of elided structure: Evidence from sluicing. Language and Cognitive Processes 26:1  pp. 63 ff. DOI logo
Frank, Robert
2004. Restricting grammatical complexity. Cognitive Science 28:5  pp. 669 ff. DOI logo
Hinojosa, J. A., E. M. Moreno & P. Ferré
2020. Affective neurolinguistics: towards a framework for reconciling language and emotion. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 35:7  pp. 813 ff. DOI logo
McRae, Ken, Mary Hare, Jeffrey L. Elman & Todd Ferretti
2005. A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory & Cognition 33:7  pp. 1174 ff. DOI logo
Morgan, Adam M. & Victor S. Ferreira
2021. Beyond input: Language learners produce novel relative clause types without exposure.. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 33:5  pp. 483 ff. DOI logo
Novick, Jared M., John C. Trueswell & Sharon L. Thompson-Schill
2005. Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 5:3  pp. 263 ff. DOI logo
Spehar, Brent, Stacey Goebel & Nancy Tye-Murray
2015. Effects of Context Type on Lipreading and Listening Performance and Implications for Sentence Processing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 58:3  pp. 1093 ff. DOI logo
Tabor, Whitney & Sean Hutchins
2004. Evidence for Self-Organized Sentence Processing: Digging-In Effects.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30:2  pp. 431 ff. DOI logo
Trueswell, John C. & Lila R. Gleitman
2012. Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics,  pp. 635 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 9 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.