Chapter 2
Towards an integrated model of grammatical encoding and decoding in SLA
In this chapter, I consider the interface between production and comprehension
in second language acquisition (SLA). I argue that the two processes rely on (partially)
shared resources and propose an integrated encoding-decoding model of SLA. The core of the
model is a single syntactic processor underlying both second language (L2) grammatical
encoding and decoding. The model also includes a means of accounting for the interaction of
shared grammatical resources and key semantic aspects such as lexical semantics and event
probability on the comprehension process. My claims are tested in an empirical study
focussing on the L2 acquisition of English passive constructions by 24 learners with an L1
German background at different stages of L2 acquisition. The results of the data analysis
provide support for the notion of a single syntactic processor in L2 acquisition.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.A Shared Grammatical Workspace in language processing
- 3.The perspective on SLA – Processability Theory & the Multiple Constraints
Hypothesis
- 1Argument structure
- 2Functional structure
- 3Constituent structure
- 4The lexicon
- 4.An integrated perspective on encoding and decoding in SLA
- 5.The acquisition of the passive in comprehension and production
- 5.1The passive in LFG and PT
- 5.2Minimal requirements in production and comprehension
- 6.The study – Research design and results
- 6.1Tasks for data elicitation
- 6.2Data analysis
- 6.3Results
- 6.3.1Stages of acquisition
- 6.3.2Production data: Passive
- 6.3.3Comprehension data: Passive
- 6.3.4Relating comprehension and production data
- 7.Conclusion
-
Notes
-
References
References (90)
References
Akers, G. (1981). Phonological variation in the Jamaican continuum. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.
Andersen, R. W. (1978). An implicational model for second language research. Language Learning 28(2), 221–282.
Bailey, C.-J. N. (1973a). Variation and linguistic theory. Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Bailey, C.-J. N. (1973b). The patterning of linguistic variation. In R. Bailey & J. Robinson (Eds.). Varieties of present-day English (pp. 156–187). New York: Macmillan.
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & V. Venza. (1984). A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation
strategies. Child Development 55(2), 341–354.
Bayley, R. (1999). The primacy of aspect hypothesis revisited: Evidence from language shift. Southwest Journal of Sociolinguistics 18(2), 1–22.
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–362). New York, NY: Wiley.
Bickerton, D. (1971). Inherent variability and variable rules. Foundations of Language 7(4), 457–492.
Bickerton, D. (1973). The nature of a creole continuum. Language 49(3), 640–669.
Bloom, L. (1974). Talking, understanding, and thinking. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, retardation and intervention (pp. 285–311). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Bresnan, J. (2001). Lexical-functional syntax. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Buyl, A., & Housen, A. (2013). Testing the applicability of PT to receptive grammar knowledge in early immersion education. Theoretical considerations, methodological challenges and some empirical results. In A. Flyman Mattson & C. Norrby (Eds.), Language acquisition and use in multilingual contexts (pp. 13–27). Lund: Lund University Press.
Buyl, A., & Housen, A. (2015). Developmental stages in receptive grammar acquisition: A Processability
Theory account. Second Language Research 31(4), 523–550.
Chapman, R. S., & Kohn, L. L. (1978). Comprehension strategies in two- and three-year-olds: Animate agents or
probable events? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 21(4), 746–761.
Chapman, R. S., & Miller, J. F. (1975). Word order in early two and three word utterances: Does production precede comprehension? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 18, 355–371.
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 27(1), 3–42.
Clark, E. V. (1980). Here's the top: Nonlinguistic strategies in the acquisition of orientational
terms. Child Development 51(2), 329–338.
Clark, H. H., & Malt, B. C. (1984). Psychological constraints on language: A commentary on Bresnan and Kaplan
and on Givón. In W. Kintsch, J. R. Miller, & P. G. Polson (Eds.), Methods and tactics in cognitive science (pp. 191–214). Hilldsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
DeCamp, D. (1971). Toward a generative analysis of a post-creole speech continuum. In D. Hymes (Ed.). Pidginization and creolization of languages (pp. 349–370). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Di Biase, B. (2008). Focus-on-form and development in L2 learning. In J.-U. Keßler (Ed.). Processability approaches to second language development and second language learning (pp. 197–219). Newcastle UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Dittmar, N. (1980). Ordering adult learners according to language abilities. In S. Felix (Ed.). Second language development (pp. 205–231). Tübingen: Narr.
Edwards, A. (1948). On Guttman's scale analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement 8(3–1), 313–318.
Ellis, R. (2008). Investigating grammatical difficulty in second language learning: Implications for second language acquisition research and language testing. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 18(1), 4–22.
Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11(1), 11–15.
Garrett, M. (2000). Remarks on the architecture of language processing systems. In Y. Grodzinsky & L. Shapiro (Eds.), Language and the brain: Representation and processing (pp. 31–69). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Goodenough, W. H. (1944). A technique for scale analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement 4(3), 179–190.
Guttman, L. A. (1944). A basis for scaling qualitative data. American Sociological Review 9(2), 139–150.
Guttman, L. A. (1950). The basis for scalogram analysis. In S. A. Stouffer, L. A. Guttman, E. A. Suchman, P. F. Lazarsfeld, S. A. Star, & J. A. Clausen (Eds.). Studies in social psychology in World War II: Vol. 4. Measurement and prediction (pp. 60–90). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Hakuta, K. (1982). Interaction between particles and word order in the comprehension of simple
sentences in Japanese children. Developmental Psychology 18(1), 62–76.
Hill, F. (1998). Acquisition of the passive. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 23(2), 63–70.
Hatch, E., & Lazaraton, A. (1991). The research manual: Design and statistics for applied linguistics. Rowley MA: Newbury House.
Ingram, D. (1974). The relationship between comprehension and production. In R. Schiefelbusch & L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (pp. 313–334). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Itani-Adams, Y. (2007). Lexical and grammatical development in Japanese-English bilingual first language acquisition. In F. Mansouri (Ed.). Second language acquisition research: Theory-construction and testing (pp. 173–198). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Press.
Izumi, S., & U. Lakshmanan (1998). Learnability, negative evidence and the L2 acquisition of the English passive. Second Language Research 14(1), 62–101.
Kawaguchi, S. (2008). Language typology and Processability Theory. In J.-U. Keßler (Ed.), Processability approaches to second language development and second language
learning (pp. 89–112). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.
Kawaguchi, S. (2016). Question constructions, argument mapping, and vocabulary development in English L2 by Japanese speakers: A cross-sectional study. In J.-U. Keßler, A. Lenzing, & M. Liebner (Eds.). Developing, modelling and assessing second languages (pp. 35–64). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Keatinge, D., & Keßler, J.-U. (2009). The acquisition of the passive voice in L2-English: Perception and
production. In J.-U. Keßler & D. Keatinge (Eds.), Research in second language acquisition: Empirical evidence across
languages (pp. 69–94). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.
Kempen, G. (2000). Could grammatical encoding and grammatical decoding be subserved by the same
processing module? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(1), 38–39.
Kempen, G., Olsthoorn, N., & Sprenger, S. (2012). Grammatical workspace sharing during language production and language
comprehension: Evidence from grammatical multitasking. Language and Cognitive Processes 27(3), 345–380.
Kersten, K., Rohde, A., Schelletter, C., & Steinlen, A. K. (Eds.). (2010). Bilingual preschools, Volume 1: Learning and development. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag Trier.
Labov, W. (1973). The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C.-J. Bailey, & R. W. Schuy (Eds.). New ways of analysing variation in English (pp. 340–373). Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.
Lameli, A. (2004). Hierarchies of dialect features in a diachronic view - implicational scaling of real time data. In B.-L. Gunnarsson, L. Bergström, G. Eklund, S. Fridell, & L. H. Hansen (Eds.). Language variation in Europe. Papers from the second international conference on language variation in Europe (ICLaVE 2), Uppsala-University, Sweden, June 12–14, 2003 (pp. 253–266). Uppsala: University Press.
Lempert, H. (1978). Extrasyntactic factors affecting passive sentence comprehension by young
children. Child Development 49(3), 694–699.
Lenzing, A. (2013). The development of the grammatical system in early second language acquisition. The
Multiple Constraints Hypothesis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Lenzing, A. (2015). Exploring regularities and dynamic systems in L2 development. Language Learning 65(1), 89–122.
Lenzing, A. (forthc.). The production-comprehension interface in second language acquisition. An
integrated encoding-decoding model. London: Bloomsbury.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking. From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Maratsos, M. P. (1974). Children who get worse at understanding the passive: A replication of Bever. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 3(1), 65–74.
Maratsos, M., Fox, D., Becker, J., & Chalkley, M. (1985). Semantic restrictions on children’s passives. Cognition 19(2), 167–191.
Marinis, T. (2007). Proceedings of the 2nd conference on generative approaches to language
acquisition North America (GALANA): On-line processing of passives in L1 and L2
children. In A. Belikova, L. Meroni, & U. Umeda (Eds.), 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition (pp. 265–276). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Marinis, T., & Saddy, D. (2013). Parsing the passive: Comparing children with Specific Language Impairment to
sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition 20(2), 155–179.
McIver, J. P., & Carmines, E. G. (1981). Unidimensional scaling. Beverly Hills CA: Sage.
Meints, K. (1999). Typizitätseffekte im Erwerb des englischen Passivs. Eine empirische
Untersuchung. Wiesbaden: DUV.
Meisel, J., Clahsen, H., & Pienemann, M. (1981). On determining developmental sequences in natural second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 3(2), 109–135..
Menenti, L., Gierhan, S. M., Segaert, K., & Hagoort, P. (2011). Shared language: Overlap and segregation of the neuronal infrastructure for
speaking and listening revealed by functional MRI. Psychological Science 22(9), 1173–1182.
Menzel, H. (1953). A new coefficient for scalogram analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly 17(2), 268–280.
Nagy, N., Moisset, C., & Sankoff, G. (1996). On the acquisition of variable phonology in L2. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 3(1), 111–126.
Pica, T., Kanagy, R., & Falodun, J. (1993). Choosing and using communicative tasks for second language
instruction. In G. Crookes & S. M. Gass (Eds.), Tasks and language learning: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 9–34). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Pickering, M., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during
comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Science 11(3), 105–110.
Pickering, M., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and
comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4), 329–347.
Pienemann, M., & Mackey, A. (1993). An empirical study of children’s ESL development and Rapid
Profile. In P. McKay (Ed.), ESL development. Language and literacy in schools (Vol. 2, pp. 115–259). Commonwealth of Australia and National Languages and Literacy Institute of Australia.
Pienemann, M., Di Biase, B., & Kawaguchi, S. (2005). Extending Processability Theory. In M. Pienemann (Ed.), Cross-linguistic aspects of Processability Theory (pp. 199–251). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Pienemann, M., & Lenzing, A. (2015). Processability Theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (2nd ed., pp. 159–179). New York, NY: Routledge.
Rickford, J. R. (1979). Variation in a creole continuum: Quantitative and implicational approaches. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Pennsylvania.
Rickford, J. (2002). Implicational scales. In J. K. Chambers, P. Trudgill, & N. Schilling-Estes (Eds.). The handbook of language variation and change (pp. 142–167). Oxford: Blackwell.
Roos, J. (2007) Spracherwerb und Sprachproduktion: Lernziele und Lernergebnisse im Englischunterricht der Grundschule. Tübingen: Narr.
Ruder, K. F., & Finch, A. (1987). Toward a cognitive-based model of language production. In H. W. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.). Psycholinguistic models of production (pp. 109–138). Westport CT: Ablex.
Sanford, A., & Sturt, P. (2002). Depth of processing in language comprehension: Not noticing the
difference. Trends in Cognitive Science 6(9), 382–386.
Sankoff, G. (1973). Above and beyond phonology in variable rules. In C.-J. Bailey, & R. W. Schuy (Eds.). New ways of analysing variation in English (pp. 44–61). Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.
Sayehli, S. (2013). Developmental perspectives on transfer in third language acquisition. Lund: Travaux de l'Institute de Linguistique de Lund.
Segaert, K., Menenti, L., Weber, K., Petersson, K. & Hagoort, P. (2012). Shared syntax in language production and language comprehension – an fMRI
study. Cerebral Cortex 22(7), 1662–1670.
Slobin, D. (1966). Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in child and
adulthood. Journal of Verbal Language and Verbal Behavior 5(3), 19–227.
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A
meta-analysis. Language Learning 60(2), 263–308.
Spinner, P. (2011). Second language assessment and morpho-syntactic development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33 (4), 529–561.
Spinner, P. (2013). Language production and reception: A Processability Theory
study. Language Learning 63(4), 704–739.
Spinner, P., & Jung, S. (2017). Production and comprehension in Processability Theory: A self-paced reading study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40(2), 1–24. doi: .
Steinlen, A. K., Håkansson, G., Housen, A., & Schelletter, C. (2010). Receptive L2 grammar knowledge development in bilingual
preschools. In K. Kersten, A. Rohde, C. Schelletter, & A. K. Steinlen (Eds.), Bilingual preschools, Volume 1: Language and development (pp. 69–100). Trier: Wissenschaftliger Verlag Trier.
Tomlin, R. S. (1997). Mapping conceptual representations into linguistic representations: The role of attention in grammar. In J. Nuyts, & E. Pederson (Eds.). Language and conceptualization (pp. 162–189). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trofimovich, P., Gatbonton, E., & Segalowitz, N. (2007). A dynamic look at L2 phonological learning: Seeking processing explanations for implicational phenomena. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 29(3), 407–448.
Trudgill, P. (1986). Dialects in contact. Oxford: Blackwell.
VanPatten, B. (1984). Morphemes and processing strategies. In F. Eckman, L. Bell, & D. Nelson (Eds.), Universals of second language acquisition (pp. 88–98). Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.
VanPatten, B. (1996). Input processing and grammar instruction: Theory and research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
VanPatten, B. (2015). Input processing in adult SLA. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition. An introduction (2nd ed., pp. 113–134). New York, NY: Routledge.
Wang, K. (2011). The fish was eaten: The acquisition of the English passive construction by Chinese
learners of ESL. Saarbrücken, Germany: LAP Lambert Academic Publishing.
Watermeyer, M. (2010). Ausagieren von Sätzen versus Satz-Bild-Zuordnung. Vergleich zweier Methoden zur Untersuchung des Sprachverständnisses anhand von semantisch reversiblen Sätzen mit Objektvoranstellung bei drei- und fünfjährigen Kindern. Unpublished diploma thesis. Potsdam University.
Cited by (3)
Cited by three other publications
Liu, Bo, Bruno Di Biase & Satomi Kawaguchi
Schmiderer, Katrin & Barbara Hinger
2023.
L’INTERLINGUA PRODUTTIVA E RICETTIVA DI STUDENTI DI ITALIANO LS IN UN CONTESTO DI SCUOLA SECONDARIA AUSTRIACA.
Italiano LinguaDue 15:2
► pp. 43 ff.
Keßler, Jörg‐U. & Anke Lenzing
2022.
Grammar in Foreign and Second Language Classes. In
The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching,
► pp. 1 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.