Part of
Enabling Human Conduct: Studies of talk-in-interaction in honor of Emanuel A. Schegloff
Edited by Geoffrey Raymond, Gene H. Lerner and John Heritage
[Pragmatics & Beyond New Series 273] 2017
► pp. 327349
References (26)
References
Armstrong, W.H. (1928). Rossel Island. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness: Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Dixon, R.M.W. (1971). ‘A Method of Semantic Description’, in D. Steinberg and L. Jakobovits (eds) Semantics, pp. 436–71. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ford, C., Fox, B. and Thompson, S. (2002). ‘Social Interaction and Grammar’, in M. Tomasello (ed.) The New Psychology of Language, vol. II, pp. 119–44. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1979). ‘Footing’, Semiotica 25: 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational Organization. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
. (ed.) (2003). Conversation and Brain Damage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1988). Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S.C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1987). ‘Minimization and Conversational Inference’, in M. Pappi and J. Verschueren (eds) The Pragmatic Perspective, pp. 61–129. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1992). ‘Activity Types and Language’, in P. Drew and J. Heritage (eds) Talk at Work, pp. 66–100. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
. (2000). Presumptive Meanings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2004). ‘On the “Human Interactional Engine”’, paper prepared for Wenner Gren Conference , Octobe.
. (in preparation) 2019. ‘Clans and Kinterms on Rossel Island’.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1958). Structural Anthropology. London: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Lounsbury, F. (1969). ‘A Formal Account of the Crow- and Omaka-Type Kinship Terminologies’, in S. Tyler (ed.) Cognitive Anthropology, pp. 212–54. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Meyer Fortes, W.W. (1969). Kinship and the Social Order: The Legacy of Lewis Henry Morgan. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. and Thompson, S. (eds) (1996). Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radcliffe-Brown, A.R. (1952). Structure and Function in Primitive Society. London: Cohen and West.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. (1972). ‘On the Analyzability of Stories by Children’, in J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds) Directions in Sociolinguistics, pp. 346–80. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. and Schegloff, E.A. (1979). ‘Two Preferences in the Organization of Reference to Persons in Conversation and their Interaction’, in G. Psathas (ed.) Everyday Language, pp. 15–21. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E.A. (1987). ‘Between Micro and Macro’, in J. Alexander (ed.) The Micro–Macro Link, pp. 207–34. Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
. (1995). ‘Some Practices for Referring to Persons in Talk-in-Interaction: A Partial Sketch of a Systematics’, in B. Fox (ed.) Studies in Anaphora, pp. 437–85. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B. and Ochs, E. (eds) (1986). Language Socialization across Cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar