Article published In:
Discourse-pragmatic markers, fillers and filled pauses: Pragmatic, cognitive, multimodal and sociolinguistic perspectives
Edited by Kate Beeching, Grant Howie, Minna Kirjavainen and Anna Piasecki
[Pragmatics & Cognition 29:2] 2022
► pp. 370393
References
Aller Media ltd
2014The Suomi 24 Sentences Corpus (2016H2) [text corpus]. Kielipankki. Retrieved from [URL]
Arnold, Jennifer E., Michael K. Tanenhouse, Rebecca J. Altmann & Maria Fagnano
2004‘The old and thee, uh, new’: Disfluency and reference resolution. Psychological Science 15(9). 578–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J. & Mandana Seyfeddinipur
2010The role of fillers in listener attributions for speaker disfluency. Language and Cognitive Processes 251. 441–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker
2015Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 671. 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beattie, Geoffrey & Brian Butterworth
1979Contextual probability and word frequency as determinants of pauses in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech 221. 201–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad & Edward Finegan
1999Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Boomer, Donald. S. & Allen T. Dittmann
1962Hesitation pauses and juncture pauses in speech. Language and Speech 5(4). 215–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bortfeld, Heather, Silvia D. Leon, Jonathan E. Bloom, Michael F. Schober & Susan E. Brennan
2001Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role and gender. Language and Speech 441. 123–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bosker, Hans R., Hugo Quené, Ted Sanders & Nivja H. de Jong
2014Native ums elicit prediction of low-frequency referents, but non-native ums do not. Journal of Memory and Language 751. 104–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, Susan E. & Michael F. Schober
2001How listeners compensate for disfluencies in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language 441. 274–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, Susan E. & Maurice Williams
1995The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language 341. 383–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan
1998The emergent lexicon. Chicago Linguistics Society 34(2). 421–435.Google Scholar
2006From usage to grammar: The minds response to repetition. Language 82(4). 323–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan & Dan. I. Slobin
1982Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58(2). 265–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, Jiahua & Zehua Chen
2008Extended Bayesian information criteria for model selection with large model spaces. Biometrika 95(3). 759–771. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Jean E. Fox Tree
2002Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition 841. 73–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Thomas Wasow
1998Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology 371. 201–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corley, Martin, Lucy MacGregor & David I. Donaldson
2007‘It’s the way that you, er, say it’: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition 1051. 658–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corley, Martin & Oliver W. Stewart
2008Hesitation disfluencies in spontaneous speech: The meaning of um . Language and Linguistics Compass 21. 589–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crible, Ludivine, Liesbeth Degand & Gaëtanelle Gilquin
Cross, Emily. S. & Deborah M. Burke
2004Do alternative names block young and older adults’ retrieval of proper names? Brain and Language 89(1). 174–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, Sacha, Denny Borsboom & Eiko I. Fried
2018Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods 50(1). 195–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Etelämäki, Marja & Minna Jaakkola
2009 Tota ja puhetilanteen todellisuus. Virittäjä 113(2). 188–212.Google Scholar
Firth, John R.
1957A synopsis of linguistic theory, 1930-1955. In Studies in linguistic analysis 1–32. Oxford: Blackwell. 1–32.Google Scholar
Fox Tree, Jean
1995The effects of false starts and repetitions on the processing of subsequent words in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language 341. 709–738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2001Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory & Cognition 291. 320–326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, Jean E.
2002Interpreting pauses and ums at turn exchanges. Discourse Processes 34(1). 37–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre & José Medina
1998Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65(2–3). 263–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele
2006Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, Frieda
1968Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Götz, Sandra
2013Fluency in native and non-native English speech. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, David W.
1998Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Language and Cognition 11. 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haakana, Markku & Laura Visapää
2014Eiku – korjauksen partikkeli? Virittäjä 118(1). 41–71.Google Scholar
Hakulinen, Auli, Maria Vilkuna, Riitta Korhonen, Vesa Koivisto, Tarja R. Heinonen & Irja Alho
2004Iso suomen kielioppi. SKS:n toimituksia 950. Helsinki: Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura.Google Scholar
Harris, Zellig S.
1954Distributional structure. Word 10(2–3). 146–162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Irvine, Christina A., Inge-Marie Eigsti & Deborah A. Fein
2016 Uh, um, and autism: Filler disfluencies as pragmatic markers in adolescents with optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46(3). 1061–1070. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janková, Jana & Sara van de Geer
2018Inference for high-dimensional graphical models. In Marloes Maathuis, Mathias Drton, Steffen Lauritzen & Martin Wainwright (eds.), Handbook of graphical models, 325–350. Florida: CRC Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jansson-Verkasalo, Eira, Maarit Silvén, Iris Lehtiö & Kurt Eggers
2021Speech disfluencies in typically developing Finnish-speaking children: Preliminary results. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 351. 707–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, Minna, Ludivine Crible & Kate Beeching
2022Are filled pauses represented as linguistic items? Investigating the effect of exposure on the perception and production of um . Language and Speech 651. 263–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjellmer, Göran
2003Hesitation. In defence of er and erm . English Studies 841. 170–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kosmala, Loulou & Ludivine Crible
2022The dual status of filled pauses: Evidence from genre, proficiency and co-occurrence. Language and Speech 651. 216–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laakso, Minna & Marjo Lehtola
2003Sanojen hakeminen afaattisen henkilön ja läheisen keskustelussa. Puhe ja Kieli 231. 1–24.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald
2000A dynamic usage-based model. In Michael Barlow, Suzanne Kemmer (eds.) Usage-based models of language, 1–36. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Levelt, Willem J. M.
1983Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition 141. 41–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loy, Jia E., Hannah Rohde & Martin Corley
2017Effects of disfluency in online interpretation of deception. Cognitive Science 411. 1434–1456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loy, Jia. E., Hannah Rohde & Martin Corley
2018Cues to lying may be deceptive: Speaker and listener behaviour in an interactive game of deception. Journal of Cognition 1(1). 42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maclay, Howard & Charles E. Osgood
1959Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech. Word 151. 19–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGregor, Karla K. & Rex R. Hadden
2020Brief report: Um fillers distinguish children with and without ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 50(5). 1816–1821. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menn, Lise & Loraine K. Obler
1990Cross-language data and theories of agrammatism. In Lise Menn & Loraine K. Obler (eds.), Agrammatic aphasia: A cross-language narrative sourcebook (vol. 21), 1369–1389. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, Melanie
2019Artificial intelligence: A guide for thinking humans. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Newman, Mark E. J.
2010Networks: An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norrick, Neal R.
2015Interjections. In Karin Aijmer & Christoph Rühlemann (eds.), Corpus pragmatics: A handbook, 291–325. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Penttilä, Nelly & Anna-Maija Korpijaakko-Huuhka
2019Disfluencies in typical Finnish-speaking adults. The Phonetician 1161. 28–41.Google Scholar
Penttilä, Nelly, Anna-Maija Korpijaakko-Huuhka & Judit Bona
2022Disfluency clusters in typical and atypical Finnish adult speech: A pilot study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 361. 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rendle-Short, Johanna
2004Showing structure: Using um in the academic seminar. Pragmatics 141. 479–498.Google Scholar
Reynolds, Allan & Allan Paivio
1968Cognitive and emotional determinants of speech. Canadian Journal of Psychology 22(3). 164–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A.
2010Some other uh(m)s . Discourse Processes 471. 130–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schnadt, Michael J. & Martin Corley
2006The influence of lexical, conceptual and planning based factors on disfluency production. Language 212(2). 8–13.Google Scholar
Schneider, Ulrike
2014Frequency, hesitations and chunks: A usage-based study of chunking in English. Freiburg: Albert-Ludwigs-Universität PhD dissertation.
Shriberg, Elizabeth E.
1994Preliminaries to a theory of speech disfluencies. California: University of California at Berkeley PhD dissertation.
Smith, Vicki L. & Herbert H. Clark
1993On the course of answering questions. Journal of Memory and Language 321. 25–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorjonen, Marja-Leena & Minna Laakso
2005Katko vai eiku? Itsekorjauksen aloitustavat ja vuorovaikutustehtävät. Virittäjä 109(2). 244–271.Google Scholar
Swerts, Marc
1998Filled pauses as markers of discourse structure. Journal of Pragmatics 301. 485–496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swerts, Marc & Emiel Krahmer
2005Audiovisual prosody and feeling of knowing. Journal of Memory and Language 53(1). 81–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tannenbaum, Percy H., Frederick Williams & Carolyn S. Hillier
1965Word predictability in the environments of hesitations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 4(2). 134–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tibshirani, Robert
1996Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 58(1). 267–288.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael
2003Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tottie, Gunnel
2011 Uh and um as sociolinguistic markers in British English. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 161. 173–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2015 Uh and um in British and American English: Are they words? Evidence from co-occurrence with pauses. In Rena Torres Cacoullos, Nathalie Dion & André Lapierre (eds.), Linguistic variation: Confronting fact and theory, 38–54. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
2016Planning what to say: Uh and um among pragmatic markers. In Gunther Kaltenböck, Evelien Keizer & Arne Lohmann (eds.), Outside the clause: Form and function of extra-clausal constituents, 97–122. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2017From pause to word: Uh, um and er in written American English. English Language & Linguistics 231. 105–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, Michael
2001The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 301. 37–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition 921. 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiological model of language learning, knowledge and use. In Gregory Hickok & Steven Small (eds.), The neurobiology of language, 953–968. Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
University of Turku, Department of Finnish and Finno-Ugric Languages
2017ArkiSyn Database of Finnish Conversational Discourse, Helsinki Korp Version [speech corpus]. Kielipankki. Retrieved from [URL]
Watanabe, Michiko, Keikichi Hirose, Yasuharu Den & Nobuaki Minematsu
2008Filled pauses as cues to the complexity of upcoming phrases for native and non-native listeners. Speech Communication 501. 81–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar